/EC-NES_Final_Report_2010-4

Page 1

Children
and
Nature
Network

 Project
Grow
Outside
 
 American
beliefs
associated
with
encouraging
 children’s
nature
experience
opportunities
 Development
and
Application
of
the
EC‐NES
Scale

18
June
2010
 Prepared
for:

Children
and
Nature
Network
 Prepared
by:
 John
Fraser,
PhD,
AIA
 Director,
ILI
New
York
 Joe
E.
Heimlich,
PhD
 Director,
ILI
Ohio
 Victor
Yocco,
 Research
Associate

Institute for Learning Innovation 3168 Braverton St., Suite 280, Edgewater, MD 21037 t: 410-956-5144 f: 410-956-5144 www.ilinet.org Understanding, fostering and promoting lifelong learning


This
survey
instrument
and
the
associated
methods
for
data
collection
and
analysis
are
copyright
 Institute
 for
 Learning
 Innovation.
 This
 instrument
 was
 developed
 for
 the
 Children
 and
 Nature
 Network
 (C&NN),
 www.childrenandnature.org,
 through
 funding
 provided
 by
 the
 W.
 K.
 Kellogg
 Foundation.
 Researchers,
 evaluators,
 academics
 and
 those
 interested
 in
 fostering
 childhood
 nature
experiences
are
encouraged
to
use
this
instrument
in
its
entirety
to
explore
the
changing
 attitudes
and
beliefs
related
to
supporting
increased
access
to
nature
by
children.
The
copyright
 holders
offer
this
instrument
without
charge
on
the
condition
that
all
reports
credit
the
original
 copyright
 holders
 and
 the
 Children
 &
 Nature
 Network,
 and
 that
 no
 changes
 are
 made
 to
 the
 instrument
or
data
collection
procedures
outlined
herein.
 
 Copyright
©
2010
Institute
for
Learning
Innovation
 Institute
for
Learning
Innovation
 3168
Braverton
St.,
Suite
280
 Edgewater,
MD
20137
 ILI
Reports:
#20100226
 
 Recommended
citation:



 Fraser,
J.
Heimlich,
J.
E.
&Yocco,
V.
(2010).
Report
Number20100226:
American
beliefs
 associated
 with
 increasing
 children’s
 opportunities
 for
 experiences
 in
 nature.


 Edgewater,
MD:
Institute
for
Learning
Innovation.

Acknowledgements:

 The
 data
 used
 in
 the
 development
 of
 the
 Encouraging
 Children’s
 Nature
 Experiences
 Scale
 (EC‐NES)
 was
 collected
 by
 over
 150
 C&NN
 volunteers
 from
 across
the
USA
in
communities
large
and
small.
Their
efforts
helped
to
ensure
 that
the
diversity
of
life
in
America
today
was
taken
into
account,
and
that
the
 items
 used
 in
 the
 scales
 could
 account
 for
 this
 variation.
 The
 authors
 and
 sponsors
wish
to
thank
these
volunteers
for
their
commitment
to
this
project
 and
 the
 time
 they
 gave
 to
 help
 advance
 the
 movement
 on
 behalf
 of
 children
 everywhere.

About
the
Institute
for
Learning
Innovation:
 Established
in
1986
as
an
independent
non‐governmental
not‐for‐profit
learning
research
and
development
 organization,
 the
 Institute
 for
 Learning
 Innovation
 is
 dedicated
 to
 changing
 the
 world
 of
 education
 and
 learning
by
understanding,
facilitating,
advocating
and
communicating
about
free‐choice
learning
across
the
 life
span.
The
Institute
provides
leadership
in
this
area
by
collaborating
with
a
variety
of
free‐choice
learning
 institutions
 such
 as
 museums,
 other
 cultural
 institutions,
 public
 television
 stations,
 libraries,
 hospitals,
 community‐based
organizations
such
as
scouts
and
the
YWCA,
scientific
societies
and
humanities
councils,
 as
 well
 as
 schools
 and
 universities.
 These
 collaborations
 strive
 to
 advance
 understanding,
 facilitate
 and
 improve
 the
 learning
 potential
 of
 these
 organizations
 by
 incorporating
 free‐choice
 learning
 principles
 in
 their
work.

Institute for Learning Innovation

2

Results
of
the
2010
C&NN
EC‐NES
National
Survey


Executive
Summary
 National
Attitudes
 This
study
of
American
adults’
attitudes
towards
children’s
experiences
in
nature
was
based
on
survey
 data
from
2,138
people
who
participated
in
an
independently
commissioned,
online
consumer
survey
in
 February
2010.



 The
Encouraging
Children’s
Nature
Experiences
Scale
(EC‐NES)
was
created
to
assess
adult
attitudes
and
 beliefs
surrounding
encouragement
of
children’s
nature
experiences.
While
a
great
deal
of
empirical
 research
has
already
been
undertaken
to
demonstrate
the
value
and
impact
of
these
experiences,
not
 all
of
the
research
has
been
adopted
by
the
public.
The
EC‐NES
scale
was
designed
as
a
benchmarking
 tool
to
assess
beliefs
about
the
value
of
these
experiences
in
four
domains:

 •

General
social
acceptance
of
types
of
benefits
that
accrue
for
children
when
they
are
in
nature,
 referred
to
as
“normative
beliefs”
measured
as:
Healthiness;
Cognitive/emotional
Growth;
 Perceived
Need;
Emotional
Well‐being;
Enhanced
Skills;
and
Appreciation
of
Nature.

Adult
control
beliefs,
or
the
belief
that
actions
by
participants
will
promote
or
restrict
children’s
 opportunities
for
nature
experience
and
measured
with
two
scales:
Adult
Priorities
and
Need
for
 Child
Safety.

Adult
behavioral
beliefs,
or
the
beliefs
that
specific
adult
behaviors
will
encourage
or
discourage
 children’s
nature
experiences,
measured
as:
Storytelling
and
Adult
Effort/Risk.
 and

Intentionality
measured
as
a
commitment
to
participate
in
activities
that
increase
children’s
 opportunities
for
nature
experiences.

The
EC‐NES
scale
was
developed
based
on
factor
analysis
of
responses
to
169
items
representing
results
 of
empirical
research
on
the
value
of
children’s
nature
experiences,
organized
into
the
core
domains
 that
have
been
demonstrated
as
predictive
of
behavior
according
to
Ajzen’s
theory
of
planned
behavior
 (Ajzen
1991).

 The
demographic
data
were
collected
in
order
to
analyze
for
race,
ethnic,
education
and
income
 variations,
although
the
sample
itself
over‐represented
the
white/Caucasian
populations.
This
variation
 was
attributed
to,
in
part,
the
desire
to
collect
data
from
six
focal
states
where
the
Children
and
Nature
 Network
has
focused
efforts.
Analysis
revealed
only
minor
variations
in
American
attitudes
based
on
 race/ethnicity.
 Overview
of
Results:
 •

American
adults
generally
believe
that
childhood
experiences
in
nature
are
important
for
all
 children,
and
very
strongly
support
the
claim
that
they
personally
intend
to
support
children
in
 having
these
experiences.

Normative
Beliefs:
Adults
generally
believe
that
all
nature
experiences
benefit
children,
and
 tend
to
believe
more
strongly
in
the
contribution
of
nature
experiences
to
physical
 development,
socialization,
and
appreciation
of
nature
than
they
believe
in
the
contribution
to
 mental
and
emotional
well‐being.

Control
Beliefs:
Adults
very
strongly
support
children’s
experiences
with
nature
but
have
great
 concern
over
the
safety
of
children
in
certain
environments.
While
they
support
nature
 experiences
in
general,
the
adults
are
very
concerned
about
children’s
safety
in
wild
places,

American
Attitudes
Toward
Children’s
Nature
Experiences

3
































































June
2010


especially
areas
described
as
woods,
waterways
such
as
creeks
and
ponds,
or
open
fields
and
 hills.
These
concerns
limit
adults’
willingness
to
allow
children
to
participate
in
unsupervised
 play
or
free
exploration
even
if
the
adults
had
these
experiences
when
they
were
children.

 •

Behavioral
Beliefs:
Adults
are
willing
to
make
the
effort
to
encourage
children
to
have
nature
 experiences
in
spite
of
the
perceived
risk
of
injury
to
these
children,
but
do
not
appear
to
model
 these
behaviors
through
personal
storytelling
or
mentoring
behavior
based
on
their
own
 experiences.

Demographic
Variations:
 •

Parents
with
children
in
their
home
are
more
likely
to
support
all
beliefs
associated
with
 encouraging
children’s
nature
experiences
outside
the
home,
with
females
agreeing
more
 strongly
than
males
with
all
concepts.

Minority
communities,
including
African
American,
Latino
and
Asian/Pacific
Islander,
rate
the
 importance
of
nature
experiences
for
their
children
at
a
lower
level
than
Caucasian/White
 Americans,
but
there
is
still
general
agreement
that
these
experiences
are
a
necessary
part
of
 child
development.
American
Indian/First
Nations
peoples
tend
to
support
these
concepts
more
 strongly
than
all
of
the
other
ethnic
groups.

Some
adults
believe
that
nature
is
quite
far
from
their
homes,
even
though
they
claim
to
live
in
 small
towns
or
relatively
undeveloped
parts
of
America.
Fully
16%
of
respondents
believe
that
 “nature”
is
more
than
30
minutes
from
their
residence.
This
perception
of
the
“nature
of
 nature”
may
lead
people
to
believe
that
some
nature
is
not
worthy
for
their
children’s
activities
 and
may
be
a
perceptual
barrier
that
can
be
easily
overcome.

Adults
with
higher
incomes
and
higher
levels
of
academic
achievement
are
also
more
likely
to
 agree
more
strongly
that
nature
experiences
are
important
for
children.

Conclusions:
 •

Although
Americans
appear
to
be
well‐intentioned
about
supporting
children’s
nature
contact,
 there
are
important
deficits
in
beliefs
around
the
benefits
of
nature
experiences
that
may
pose
a
 limit
on
whether
they
are
willing
to
act
on
these
beliefs.
These
external
factors
may
be
 overcome
through
efforts
of
a
national
campaign
targeted
at
redressing
these
beliefs.

Helping
adults
learn
that
their
mentoring
activities,
such
as
personal
storytelling
about
their
 positive
nature
experiences
as
children,
may
increase
the
likelihood
that
children
will
desire
to
 have
nature
experiences.

Because
adults
are
extremely
concerned
about
the
risks
to
children
associated
with
free‐flowing
 streams,
woods,
mountains
and
wild
nature,
a
two‐pronged
approach
around
talking
about
the
 value
of
more
proximate
nature
experiences
is
advised.
The
first
prong
might
address
the
value
 of
free‐play
in
green
spaces
and
planned
nature
places
to
increase
adults’
support
for
children’s
 nature
contact.
A
secondary
effort
focusing
on
the
safety
of
accompanied
experiences
to
more
 wild
nature
areas
may
be
more
acceptable
to
the
majority
of
Americans.

Lastly,
helping
to
leverage
American
Indian/First
Nations
people’s
stronger
support
for
the
value
 of
children’s
nature
experiences
may
offer
an
opportunity
for
links
to
an
American
land
ethic
 that
can
increase
the
appeal
of
children’s
nature
experiences
for
all
Americans.

Institute for Learning Innovation

4

Results
of
the
2010
C&NN
EC‐NES
National
Survey


Table
of
Contents
 Executive
Summary................................................................................................................. 3
 National
Attitudes........................................................................................................................... 3
 Overview
of
Results:........................................................................................................................ 3
 Demographic
Variations: ................................................................................................................ 4
 Conclusions: .................................................................................................................................... 4
 Introduction............................................................................................................................ 7
 Method................................................................................................................................... 8
 Operational
Definitions
for
the
Constructs: ....................................................................................9
 Healthiness...................................................................................................................................... 9
 Cognitive
and
Emotional
Growth .................................................................................................... 9
 Emotional
Well‐being.................................................................................................................... 10
 Enhanced
Skills.............................................................................................................................. 10
 Appreciation
of
Nature.................................................................................................................. 11
 Priority........................................................................................................................................... 11
 Safety ............................................................................................................................................ 12
 Storytelling .................................................................................................................................... 12
 Effort/Risk ..................................................................................................................................... 13
 Intentionality................................................................................................................................. 13
 Scale
Validity
and
Reliability ........................................................................................................ 14
 Findings ................................................................................................................................ 16
 Beliefs
in
the
Value
of
Childhood
Nature
Experiences................................................................... 16
 Variations
in
Beliefs
by
Demographic
Variables............................................................................ 18
 Secondary
Factor
Analysis............................................................................................................. 23
 Assessment
of
Age,
Income,
Education
and
State
of
Residence ................................................... 25
 Discussion
and
Recommendations ........................................................................................ 28
 Normative
Beliefs:......................................................................................................................... 28
 Control
Beliefs: .............................................................................................................................. 28
 Behavioral
Beliefs:......................................................................................................................... 29
 Perceived
Distance
to
Nature........................................................................................................ 30
 Parenthood,
Nature
and
the
Difference
between
Women
and
Men ............................................ 30
 Educational
Achievement,
Income
and
Culture ............................................................................ 31
 Age ................................................................................................................................................ 32
 Public
Policy .................................................................................................................................. 32
 Limitations
of
this
study ................................................................................................................ 32
 Conclusion ............................................................................................................................ 33
 Appendix
A: .......................................................................................................................... 34
 Encouraging
Children’s
Nature
Experiences
Scale
[EC‐NES]........................................................... 34
 Appendix
B: .......................................................................................................................... 41
 Descriptive
Statistics .................................................................................................................... 41

American
Attitudes
Toward
Children’s
Nature
Experiences

5
































































June
2010


Institute for Learning Innovation

6

Results
of
the
2010
C&NN
EC‐NES
National
Survey


Introduction
 The
Children
&
Nature
Network
(C&NN)
Project
Grow
Outside
is
a
three‐year
campaign
to
accelerate
the
 effectiveness
of
the
children
and
nature
movement.
The
goal
of
the
campaign
is
to
facilitate
social
 change
to
enhance
children
and
youth
health
and
well‐being
by
re‐connecting
them
to
the
natural
world
 in
their
everyday
lives.
C&NN
will
accomplish
this
goal
by
using
the
existing
knowledge
base
and
 network
to
develop
and
distribute
the
tools
necessary
to
turn
awareness
into
action,
and
build
a
 framework
for
sustainable
systemic
change
at
the
local,
national,
and
international
level.
The
ultimate
 goal
is
deep
cultural
change,
evident
in
the
“third
ring”
of
engagement
identified
as
self‐activated
and
 socially‐networked
participation
by
individuals,
families,
and
communities.
The
primary
aim
of
the
 project
is
to
expand
C&NN’s
ability
to
promote
fundamental
individual
and
institutional
change
and
 provide
resources
for
building
awareness,
sharing
information,
implementing
strategic
initiatives,
 identifying
and
disseminating
best
practices,
and
filling
the
gaps
in
research.
 In
the
past
few
decades,
a
substantive
body
of
literature
has
indicated
that
nature
experiences
are
an
 important
part
of
the
developmental
process
for
children
but
studies
of
childhood
activity
increasingly
 show
that
children
have
less
and
less
time
outdoors
in
nature.
While
there
may
be
many
causes
for
this
 nature
deficit,
adult
mentoring
and
priorities
surrounding
children’s
nature
experiences
may
have
a
 direct
impact
on
whether
children
are
encouraged
to
spend
time
outdoors.


 In
fall
of
2009,
the
Institute
for
Learning
Innovation
(ILI)
was
commissioned
by
C&NN
to
develop
a
 national
benchmarking
survey
to
measure
how
adults
perceive
the
value
of
children’s
nature
contact
 experiences,
and
how
those
beliefs
and
values
support
children’s
opportunities
to
have
such
 experiences.
The
resulting
survey
instrument
and
the
results
reported
here
outline
how
current
beliefs
 may
influence
the
likelihood
of
success
for
the
children
and
nature
movement,
areas
for
focus
by
 proponents
of
children’s
nature
experiences
to
help
advance
the
movement,
and
where
adult
beliefs
 and
behaviors
may
be
acting
in
opposition
to
the
goals
of
enhancing
children’s
lives
through
contact
 with
nature.
 The
psychometric
instrument
used
for
this
study
was
aimed
at
assessing
adult
beliefs
about
children
and
 nature
experiences.
It
probed
the
values
around
a
core
set
of
constructs
derived
from
the
children
and
 nature
literature,
organized
those
constructs
into
a
set
of
factors
based
on
the
well‐proven
theory
of
 planned
behavior,
and
then
measured
how
Americans
support
these
beliefs
through
responses
to
a
set
 of
agreement/disagreement
ranking
scales.
The
survey
also
collected
demographic
data
to
determine
 whether
specific
factors
were
related
to
the
psychometric
measures.
 These
data
were
particularly
important
for
benchmarking
against
current
and
changed
attitudes
within
 members
of
the
general
public
in
American
society,
for
members
of
socially
influential
organizations,
 among
pre/post
program
participants,
and
other
vectors
where
social
change
may
be
fostered.
The
final
 “master”
EC‐NES
questionnaire
has
been
demonstrated
to
be
a
reliable
instrument
that
can
be
deployed
 every
five
years
to
track
the
state
of
change
in
social
discourse
to
determine
the
overall
effectiveness
of
 the
children
and
nature
movement
in
American
society,
to
measure
where
values
have
changed
and
to
 compare
and
map
how
these
national
level
data
are
represented
in
communities
across
the
nation
and
 potentially
other
parts
of
the
world.

American
Attitudes
Toward
Children’s
Nature
Experiences

7
































































June
2010


Method
 In
order
to
develop
a
reliable
set
of
scales,
the
ILI
team
undertook
a
two‐phase
development
process.
 The
first
phase
consisted
of
an
expert
review
of
the
constructs
(that
is,
factors
that
predict
types
of
 beliefs)
developed
from
a
broad
range
of
theoretical
frameworks
and
research
findings
that
describe
the
 value
of
nature
experiences
to
children,
and
whether
a
proposed
set
of
items
accurately
reflected
the
 factors
in
question.
The
second
phase
consisted
of
a
field
test
of
the
items
nominated
for
the
item
bank
 in
order
for
the
research
team
to
identify
the
most
suitable
items
for
internal
consistency
within
the
 factor.
These
data
were
then
used
to
arrive
at
a
final
instrument
that
was
deployed
through
a
national
 consumer
poll
of
2,138
people
from
across
the
USA.
 To
ensure
internal
consistency
across
items,
the
item
banks
were
developed
for
each
construct
based
on
 the
theoretical
underpinnings
from
the
literature
and
existing
scales.
The
expert
panel
of
five
 representatives
from
the
academic
community
supporting
the
Children
and
Nature
Network
considered
 whether
the
items
represented
the
constructs
based
on
the
research,
and
identified
any
factors
or
item
 dimensions
that
were
missing.
These
constructs
were
then
organized
within
the
Theory
of
Planned
 Behavior
(Ajzen,
1991)
in
order
to
determine
the
likelihood
of
adults’
willingness
to
support
children’s
 nature
experiences.
The
items
within
the
constructs
are
all
tested
and
analyzed
against
and
with
all
 other
items
in
the
construct.

This
is
done
to
identify
those
items
that
are
the
strongest
predictors
of
the
 construct.
 The
final
set
of
constructs
used
in
the
survey
were:
 Normative
Beliefs:
values
that
adults
generally
believe
to
be
true
across
their
community,
or
 self‐evident
truths.
In
this
case,
what
adults
believe
children
derive
as
benefits
from
their
 experiences
in
nature
settings.
 Healthiness
 Cognitive/Emotional
Growth
 Emotional
Well‐being
 Enhanced
Skills

 Appreciation
of
Nature
 Control
Beliefs:
beliefs
that
actions
are
within
the
control
of
the
research
participant
and
 capable
of
resulting
in
an
intended
outcome.
In
this
case,
how
much
adults
prioritize
these
 experiences
as
important
for
child
development,
and
how
the
risks
associated
with
that
 contact
might
prevent
adults
from
encouraging
children
to
spend
time
exploring
nature.
 Priority
 Child
Safety
 Behavioral
Beliefs:
beliefs
that
specific
behaviors
will
result
in
a
desired
outcome.

In
this
case,
 the
sense
of
risk
associated
with
children’s
experiences
in
nature
and
these
adults’
beliefs
 that
their
activities
and
telling
stories
about
nature
might
establish
norms
and
priorities
for
 the
children
in
their
lives.
 Storytelling
 Effort/Risk
 and
 Intentionality:
the
intention
to
engage
in
behaviors
that
a
perons
believes
will
achieve
the
 desired
result
of
encouraging
children’s
nature
experience
based
on
Ajzen’s
 recommendations
for
framing
intentionality.

Institute for Learning Innovation

8

Results
of
the
2010
C&NN
EC‐NES
National
Survey


Operational
Definitions
for
the
Constructs:
 The
following
outlines
the
specific
construct
domains
and
the
research
or
theory
on
which
those
 constructs
are
built:
 Healthiness
 Research
has
suggested
a
number
of
physical
and
developmental
benefits
children
receive
from
 spending
time
in
nature.
It
has
been
suggested
that
engaging
in
play
in
nature
allows
children
to
 explore
their
creativity
at
multiple
levels
of
development.
Children
who
engage
in
play
outdoors
are
 more
likely
to
engage
in
vigorous
physical
activity
which
has
numerous
health
benefits
and
is
 strongly
recommended
by
health
professionals.
Simply
having
nature
nearby
has
been
shown
to
 improve
recovery
from
injuries,
increase
children’s
levels
of
happiness,
and
reduce
the
occurrence
 of
diseases
such
as
asthma
and
myopia.
This
scale
measured
adults’
attitudes
toward
these
benefits
 to
children
spending
time
in
nature.
 Key
readings:
 Fjortoft,
I.
(2000).
The
Natural
Environment
as
a
Playground
for
Children:
The
Impact
of
Outdoor
 Play
Activities
in
Pre‐Primary
School
Children.
Landscape
and
Urban
Planning,
48,
83‐97.
 Kellert,
S.
&Derr,
V.
(1998).
National
study
of
outdoor
wilderness
experience.
Washington,
DC:
 Island
Press.
 Lovasi,
G.
S.,
Quinn,
J.
W.,
Neckerman,
K.
M.,
Perzanowski,
M.
S.,
&
Rundle,
A.
(2008).
Children
 living
in
areas
with
more
street
trees
have
lower
prevalence
of
asthma.
Journal
of
 Epidemiology
and
Community
Health,
62(7),
647‐649.
 Rose,
K.
A.,
Morgan,
I.
G.,
Ip,
J.,
Kifley,
A.,
Huynh,
S.,
Smith,
W.,
et
al.
(2008).
Outdoor
activity
 reduces
the
prevalence
of
myopia
in
children.
Ophthalmology,
115(8),
1279‐1285.
 Ulrich,
R.
(1984).
View
through
a
window
may
influence
recovery
from
surgery.
Science,
224
420‐ 421.

Cognitive
and
Emotional
Growth

Research
indicates
that
children
who
have
nature
experiences
demonstrate
more
advanced
 cognitive
and
emotional
development.
It
has
been
suggested
that
affinity
for
nature
may
be
 genetically
encoded,
but
this
rudimentary
understanding
requires
experience
to
fully
develop.
 Researchers
have
established
links
between
types
of
nature
experiences
including
skills
in
 categorization,
classification,
learning
through
observation,
interpretation,
inference,
and
 prediction.
This
scale
focuses
on
adult
beliefs
about
the
cognitive
and
emotional
dimensions
of
 learning
that
are
supported
by
nature
experiences
including:
learning
through
direct
contact;
self‐ directed
learning;
systems
learning;
emotional
development;
and
thinking,
remembering,
and
 problem
solving.
 Key
readings:
 Burdette,
H.
&
Whitaker,
R.(2005).
Resurrecting
free
play
in
young
children:
Looking
beyond
 fitness
and
fatness
to
attention,
affiliation,
and
affect.
Archives
of
Pediatric
Adolescent
 Medicine.159:46‐50.
 Grahn,
P.,
Martensson,
F.,Lindblad,
B.,
Nilsson,
P.,
and
Ekma,
A.
(1997).
Outdoors
at
daycare.

City
 and
Country,
145.
 Kellert,
S.
(2005).
Nature
and
childhood.
Building
for
Life:
Designing
and
Understanding
the
 Human‐Nature
Connection.
Washington.
D.C.:
Island
Press.

American
Attitudes
Toward
Children’s
Nature
Experiences

9
































































June
2010


Kellert,
S.
(2002).
Experiencing
Nature:
Affective,
Cognitive,
and
Evaluative
Development.
 Children
and
Nature:
Psychological,
Sociocultural,
and
Evolutionary
Investigations.
 Cambridge,
MA:
The
MIT
Press.
 Taylor,A.,
F.,Wiley,
A.,Kuo,
F.
E.
&
Sullivan,W.
C.
(1998).
Growing
up
in
the
inner
city:
 Greenspaces
as
places
to
grow.
Environment
&
Behavior,
30(1),
3‐27.
 Wells,
N.
(2000).
At
Home
with
Nature:
Effects
of
'Greenness'
on
Children's
Cognitive
 Functioning.
Environment
and
Behavior,
32,
775‐795.

Emotional
Well‐being
 The
factors
measured
in
this
scale
include
a
number
of
benefits
to
playing
in
nature
that
have
been
 identified
by
researchers.
Mental
benefits
that
children
may
experience
from
playing
in
nature
 include
increased
confidence,
self‐esteem,
and
independence.
Research
suggests
that
children
gain
 an
awareness
of
their
self
and
a
greater
sense
of
what
they
can
control
by
playing
in
nature.
Adult
 attitudes
towards
children
developing
a
sense
of
belonging
and
a
sense
of
community
were
also
 measured
in
this
scale.
Finally,
an
extensive
amount
of
research
has
reported
on
the
benefits
of
 outdoor
play
in
fighting
the
problem
of
childhood
obesity.
Adult
attitudes
toward
this
were
 measured
in
this
scale
as
well.
 Key
readings:
 Bell,
S.,
Hamilton,
V.,
Montarzino,
A.,
Rothnie,
H.,
Travlou,
P.,
&Alves,
S.
(2008).
Greenspace
and
 quality
of
life:
A
critical
literature
review.
Greenspace
Scotland.

 Barros,
R.
M.,
Silver,
E.
J.,
&
Stein,
R.
E.
K.
(2009).
School
recess
and
group
classroom
behavior.
 Pediatrics,
123(2),
431‐436.
 Cleland,
V.,
Crawford,
D.,
Baur,
L.
A.,
Hume,
C.,
Timperio,
A.,
&
Salmon,
J.
(2008).
A
prospective
 examination
of
children's
time
spent
outdoors,
objectively
measured
physical
activity
and
 overweight.
International
Journal
of
Obesity,
32(11),
1685‐1693.
 Ginsburg,
K.
R.
(2007).
“The
importance
of
play
in
promoting
healthy
child
development
and
 maintaining
strong
parent‐child
bonds.”
Pediatrics,
119(1),
182‐191.
 Potwarka,
L.
R.,
Kaczynski,
A.
T.,
&
Flack,
A.
L.
(2008).
Places
to
play:
association
of
park
space
and
 facilities
with
healthy
weight
status
among
children.
Journal
of
Community
Health,
33(5),
 344‐350.
 
Taylor,
A.
F.,
Kuo,
F.
E.,
&
Sullivan

W.
C.
(2001).

Coping
with
ADD:
The
surprising
connection
to
 Green
Play
Settings,
Environment
and
Behavior
33(1),
54‐77.

Enhanced
Skills
 Research
has
linked
time
spent
in
nature
with
increased
strength,
reduced
stress,
and
improved
 socialization
skills.
Many
of
these
skills
come
through
unstructured
group
play
in
which
the
children
 make
the
rules
and
resolve
the
conflicts.
This
scale
focused
on
adult
beliefs
regarding
specific
skills
 children
gain
by
time
spend
playing
in
nature.
These
included
physical
strength
and
coordination
 for
physical
benefits.
Social
benefits
included
increased
learning
to
play
with
others,
cooperation,
 and
reduced
stress.

 Key
readings:
 American
Institutes
of
Research
(2005).Effects
of
outdoor
education
programs
for
children
in
 California.
Palo
Alto,
CA.
 Kellert,
S.
(1996).
The
Value
of
Life:
Biological
Diversity
and
Human
Society.
Washington,
D.C.:
 Island
Press.
 Little,
H.
&
Wyver,
S.
(2008).
Outdoor
play
‐
does
avoiding
the
risks
reduce
the
benefits?
 Australian
Journal
of
Early
Childhood,
33(2),
33‐40.

Institute for Learning Innovation

10

Results
of
the
2010
C&NN
EC‐NES
National
Survey


O'Dea,
J.
(2003).
Why
do
kids
eat
healthful
food?
Perceived
benefits
of
and
barriers
to
healthful
 eating
and
physical
activity
among
children
and
adolescents.
Journal
of
the
American
Dietetic
 Association.103(4),
497‐501.
 Ulrich,
R.,
Simons,
Losito,
et
al.
1991
Stress
recovery
during
exposure
to
natural
and
urban
 environments.
Journal
of
Environmental
Psychology,
1991,
11,
201‐230.

Appreciation
of
Nature
 Research
with
those
who
have
a
high
degree
of
concern
for
nature
has
linked
this
concern
to
early
 childhood
experiences
in
nature.
This
construct
represents
adults’
views
on
the
role
that
nature
 plays
in
developing
relationships
with
nature
including:
appreciation
of
natural
processes;
 interrelationships
between
organisms;
care
for
wild
animals;
self‐directed
development
of
moral
 values;
and
the
concept
that
learning
itself
is
enhanced
by
experiences
in
the
natural
world.
This
 construct
spans
the
range
of
topics
from
ecoliteracy
(or
environmental
literacy)
to
concepts
of
 moral/systems
knowledge
and
its
role
in
place
attachment.
 Key
readings:

AIR
2005,
American
Institutes
for
Research.
(2005).Effects
of
outdoor
education
programs
for
 children
in
California.
 Chawla,
L.
(1998).
Research
methods
to
investigate
significant
life
experiences:
Review
and
 recommendations.
Environmental
Education
Research
4
(4):
383‐397.
 Chawla,
L.
(2006).
"Learning
to
Love
the
Natural
World
Enough
to
Protect
It,"
Barn2
57‐58.
 Finger,
M.
(1993).
Environmental
adult
learning
in
Switzerland.
Occasional
Papers
Series
No.2,
 Center
for
Adult
Education,
Teachers
College,
Columbia
University,
New
York.
 Finger,
M.
(1994).
From
kinship
to
action?
Exploring
the
relationships
between
environmental
 
 experiences,
learning,
and
behavior.
Journal
of
Social
Issues
50
(3):141‐160.
 Sia,
A.,
Hungerford,
H.
&
Tomera,
A.
(1985/86).Selected
predictors
of
responsible
environmental
 behavior.
Journal
of
Environmental
Education
17
(2):31‐40.
 Sivek,
D.
&
Hungerford,
H.
(1989/90).
Predictors
of
responsible
behavior
in
members
of
three
 Wisconsin
conservation
organizations.
Journal
of
Environmental
Education
21
(2):35‐40.
 Sobel,
D.
(2005).
Beyond
Ecophobia:
Reclaiming
the
Heart
in
Nature
Education.
The
Orion
Society,
 Great
Barrington,
MA.
 Sobel,
D.
(2008).
Childhood
and
Nature.
Stenhouse
Publishers.
Portland,
ME.
 Tanner,
T.
(Ed.)
1998.
Special
issue
on
significant
life
experiences
research.
Environmental
 Education
Research
4
(4).
 Tanner,
T.
(Ed.)
(1999).
Special
section
on
significant
life
experiences
research.
Environmental
 Education
Research
5
(2).
 Turner,
W.
R.,
Nakamura,
T.,
&
Dinetti,
M.
(2004).
Global
Urbanization
and
the
Separation
of
 Humans
from
Nature.
Bioscience,
54(6),
585‐590

Priority
 Adults
typically
influence
or
control
how
children
spend
their
time.
Therefore
what
adults
see
as
 the
priority
for
children
to
spend
their
time
doing
will
be
reflected
in
what
adults
allow
children
to
 do.
Research
suggests
that
adults
may
feel
children
do
not
respect
property
or
nature,
and
that
 children
left
outdoors
unsupervised
will
engage
in
behaviors
destructive
to
property.
Research
has
 also
suggested
adults
judge
other’s
parenting
abilities
by
how
clean
their
children
are.
Studies
 indicate
that
many
adults
feel
children
do
not
need
to
spend
a
large
quantity
of
time
outdoors,
and
 that
time
spent
engaging
in
structured
extracurricular
activities
is
time
better
spent
than
 unsupervised
time
outdoors.
If
adult
priorities
lie
with
keeping
children
supervised,
clean,
and
in
 indoor
learning
situations,
this
is
a
barrier
to
children
engaging
in
outdoor
play.

American
Attitudes
Toward
Children’s
Nature
Experiences

11
































































June
2010


Key
readings:
 Dwyer,
G.
M.,
Higgs,
J.,
Hardy,
L.
L.,
&
Baur,
L.
A.
(2008).
What
do
parents
and
preschool
staff
tell
 us
about
young
children's
physical
activity:
A
qualitative
study.
International
Journal
of
 Behavioral
Nutrition
and
Physical
Activity,
5(11).
 Hofferth,
S.
&
Sandberg,
J.
(2001).
Changes
in
American
Children’s
Time,
1981‐1997.
In
S.L.
 Hofferth&
T.J.
Owens
(Eds.),
Children
at
the
Millennium:
Where
Have
We
Come
From,
Where
 Are
We
Going?
(pp.
1‐7).
New
York:
JAI,
2001.
 Lacy,
L.
(2007).
Playday
2007,
Our
Streets
Too!
London:
Children’s
Play
Council.
 Lester,
S.,
&
Maudsley,
M.
(2006).
Play,
naturally:
A
review
of

children's
natural
play.
London:
 Children's
Play
Council.

Safety
 The
perception
of
safety
has
been
identified
as
a
key
factor
in
parents
allowing
children
to
play
in
a
 variety
of
settings.
Research
suggests
that
parents’
decisions
on
where
to
let
their
children
play
are
 strongly
influenced
by
the
parents’
comfort
level
with
that
setting.
If
parents
are
not
comfortable
in
 nature,
they
will
in
turn
not
be
comfortable
allowing
their
child
in
nature.
The
perceived
availability
 of
safe
places
to
play
outdoors
also
influences
parents’
decisions
on
where
their
child
can
play.
This
 scale
also
covers
the
perception
that
bad
people
are
more
likely
to
frequent
outdoor
areas
where
 children
are
known
to
play.
In
addition
to
the
items
tested
for
this
construct,
two
external
scales
 were
used
to
assess
perceptions
of
safety
based
on
studies
conducted
in
the
United
Kingdom.
 These
scales
assessed
places
for
childhood
free‐play
experiences
that
adults
may
have
had
when
 they
were
children
and
these
same
adults’
willingness
to
allow
children
similar
opportunities
today.

 Key
readings:
 Carver,
A.,
Timperio,
A.,
&
Crawford,
D.
(2008).
“Playing
it
safe:
The
influence
of
neighbourhood
 safety
on
children's
physical
activity
‐
A
review.”
Health
&
Place,
14(2),
217‐227.
 Gleave
J.
(2008).
Risk
and
play,
a
literature
review.
London:
Children’s
Play
Council.
 Sallis,
McKenzie,
Elder,
Broyles,
and
Nader.
(1997).
Factors
Parents
Use
in
Selecting
Play
Spaces
 for
Young
Children.
Archives
of
Pediatric
and
Adolescent
Medicine,
151(4):414‐417.
 Lacy,
L.
(2007).
Playday
2007,
Our
Streets
Too!
London:
Children’s
Play
Council.
 Valentine,
G.
(2004).
Public
Space
and
the
Culture
of
Childhood.
Ashgate
Publishing,
Surrey,
 United
Kingdom.

Storytelling
 Cultures
have
long
shared
traditions
and
passed
along
values
through
storytelling.
Storytelling
is
a
 way
for
adults
to
pass
on
their
experiences
to
children
as
well.
Researchers
have
also
discussed
the
 ability
for
storytelling
to
influence
children’s
behavior.
This
scale
measured
adults’
attitudes
toward
 and
participation
in
telling,
hearing,
and
reading
stories
about
nature.
Researchers
have
discussed
 the
importance
of
storytelling
as
an
avenue
for
humans
to
know
nature
and
to
understand
the
role
 nature
plays
in
one’s
life,
leading
to
the
belief
that
adult
storytelling
will
be
critical
to
children
 developing
positive
attitudes
towards
nature.
 Key
readings:
 Cronon,
W.
(1992).
A
place
for
stories:
Nature,
history,
and
narrative.
The
Journal
of
American
 History,
78(4),
1347‐1376.
 Colpitts,
G.
(2002).
Game
in
the
garden:
A
human
history
of
wildlife
in
Western
Canada
to
1940.
 Vancouver,
British
Columbia,
Canada:
UBC
Press.

Institute for Learning Innovation

12

Results
of
the
2010
C&NN
EC‐NES
National
Survey


Fawcett,
L.(2000).
Ethical
imagining:
Ecofeminist
possibilities
and
environmental
learning.

 Canadian
Journal
of
Environmental
Education,
5,
134‐147.
 Fawcett,
L.
(2002).
Children’s
wild
animal
stories:
questioning
inter‐species
bonds,
Canadian
 Journal
of
Environmental
Education,
7(2),
125–139.
 Zerner,
C.
(1996).
Telling
stories
about
biological
diversity.
In:
Brush
SB,
Stabinsky
D
(eds)
Valuing
 local
knowledge:
indigenous
people
and
intellectual
property
rights.
Washington,
D.C.:
Island
 Press.

Effort/Risk
 This
scale
focuses
on
adults’
beliefs
regarding
the
support
for
children’s
nature
experiences.
While
 adults
may
believe
children
learn
through
nature
experiences,
their
perceptions
of
control
often
 lead
to
a
feeling
that
allowing
children
to
play
unsupervised
in
nature
will
lead
to
negative
 outcomes
for
the
child.
Similarly
adults
may
need
to
see
allowing
children
to
play
in
nature
 modeled
by
schools
and
day
care,
relying
on
these
institutions
to
provide
guidance
in
how
they
 allow
their
children
to
spend
time.
Other
concepts
that
are
found
in
the
literature
deemed
to
be
 out
of
the
direct
control
of
parents
include
children
being
exposed
to
germs
that
cause
sickness
and
 the
costs
associated
with
a
potential
injury
incurred
through
outdoor
play.
 Key
readings:
 Bell,
A.
&
Dyment,
J.
(2006).
Grounds
for
action:
Promoting
physical
activity
through
 schoolground
greening
in
Canada.
Toronto,
ON:
Evergreen.
 Churchman,
A.
(2003).
Is
There
a
Place
for
Children
in
the
City.
Journal
of
Urban
Design,
8
(2),
99‐ 111.
 Clements,
R.
(2004).
An
Investigation
of
the
State
of
Outdoor
Play.
Contemporary
Issues
in
Early
 Childhood,
5(1)
68‐80.
 Parsad,
B.
&
Lewis,
L.
(2005).
Calories
In,
Calories
Out:
Food
and
Exercise
in
Public
Elementary
 Schools,
2005
(NCES
2006‐057).
U.S.
Department
of
Education.
Washington,
DC:
National
 Center
for
Education
Statistics,
2006.
 U.S.
Centers
for
Disease
Control
and
Prevention
(2003).Physical
Activity
Levels
Among
Children
 Aged
9
to
13
Years—United
States,
2002.MMWR
Weekly;
52(33):785‐88.

Intentionality
 This
scale
measured
participants’
expressed
intent
to
engage
in
a
number
of
activities
related
to
 influencing
children’s
nature
behaviors
and
attitudes,
and
the
participants’
likelihood
to
engage
in
 activities
promoting
children’s
access
to
nature.
Measuring
intent
is
looked
at
as
a
proxy
 measurement
for
actual
behavior;
therefore,
this
scale
provides
us
with
a
forecast
of
the
behaviors
 participants
may
engage
in
in
the
future.
Activities
that
have
been
identified
by
researchers
as
 having
a
critical
influence
on
children
spending
time
in
nature
include
taking
children
to
nature
 places
to
play,
providing
children
with
the
opportunity
to
be
outdoors,
and
teaching
children
 respect
for
nature
and
property.

 Key
readings:
 Ajzen,
I.
(1991).
The
Theory
of
Planned
Behavior.
Organizational
Behavior
and
Human
Decision
 Processes,
50(2),
179‐211.
 Ajzen,
I.,
&
Fishbein,
M.
(1980).
Understanding
Attitudes
and
Predicting
Social
Behavior.
 Englewood
Cliffs,
NJ:
Prentice‐Hall.
 Armitage,
C.
J.
&
Conner,
M.
(2001).
Efficacy
of
the
Theory
of
Planned
Behavior,
a
Meta‐analytical
 Review.British
Journal
of
Social
Psychology,
40(4),
471‐499.
 Sheeran,
P.,
Conner,
M.,
&Norman,
P.
(2001).
Can
the
theory
of
planned
behavior
explain
 patterns
of
health
behavior
change?
Health
Psychology
20(1),
12‐19.

American
Attitudes
Toward
Children’s
Nature
Experiences

13
































































June
2010


These
constructs
were
organized
within
a
framework
to
establish
how
each
aspect
of
the
belief
 structures
might
support
or
limit
an
intention
or
actual
behavior
that
supports
children’s
nature
 experiences
as
illustrated
in
Figure
1.

Figure
1:
Relationship
of
constructs
to
the
support
for
children’s
nature
contact
experiences.

Scale
Validity
and
Reliability
 Based
on
the
literature,
ILI
developed
a
set
of
169
possible
items
that
could
represent
these
constructs
 as
potential
items
for
a
final
measurement
tool.
ILI
staff
and
C&NN
volunteers
tested
these
169
 individual
items
in
communities
across
the
USA
during
December
2009
and
January
2010.
The
items
 used
to
create
the
final
scales
were
pilot
tested
together
based
on
their
relationship
to
the
factors
 making
up
the
construct
covered
by
each
scale.
Eight
groups
of
items
were
pilot
tested
and
then
 examined
by
the
researchers.
The
large
number
of
items
tested
allowed
for
researchers
to
eliminate
 many
of
the
items,
creating
scales
with
high
reliability
in
terms
of
the
relationship
between
the
items.
 Each
group
of
items
was
subjected
to
the
following
statistical
analysis:
basic
descriptive
(mean,
median,
 standard
deviation,
and
standard
error),
correlation
between
items,
exploratory
factor
analysis,
and
 scale
reliability.
 Correlations
were
examined
first
to
eliminate
items
that
were
very
highly
correlated
(i.e.,
>.850).
The
 justification
for
this
is
that
items
with
very
high
correlations
suggest
that
items
are
too
strongly
related,

Institute for Learning Innovation

14

Results
of
the
2010
C&NN
EC‐NES
National
Survey


participants
are
interpreting
the
items
very
similarly
and
therefore
a
response
to
one
of
the
correlated
 items
can
be
viewed
as
a
similar
response
to
the
others.
In
looking
at
the
groups
of
items
that
were
 related
and
highly
correlated,
the
item
that
had
the
strongest
correlations
with
the
largest
number
of
 items
was
kept.
The
items
it
was
most
strongly
correlated
with
were
eliminated.

 The
next
step
involved
eliminating
items
with
very
low
correlation
for
this
study.
There
were
a
large
 number
of
potential
items
for
each
scale
that
were
correlated
between
.500
and
.800,
therefore
the
 researchers
chose
.500
as
a
cut
off
point
for
what
would
be
considered
very
low.
Items
that
had
all
or
a
 majority
of
low
correlations
were
eliminated.
The
justification
for
this
is
that
the
items
that
were
mid‐ range
(0.500‐0.800)
in
their
correlation
would
interact
well
together
and
the
statistics
suggest
 participants’
responses
on
these
items
were
related,
which
is
a
validation
of
the
theory
that
underlies
 the
construct
covered
by
each
scale.
Low
correlation
for
an
item
between
all
or
most
of
the
other
items
 suggests
that
the
item
was
not
as
effective
at
representing
the
factor
it
was
hypothesized
to
measure.
 The
exploratory
factor
analysis
results
were
consulted
following
the
elimination
of
items
based
on
 correlation.
Each
group
of
items
initially
generated
multiple
factors.
This
was
expected
as
each
of
the
 constructs
we
are
measuring
contains
a
number
of
factors
within
them.
The
next
step
was
to
determine
 which
items
would
be
used
to
best
represent
each
factor
within
the
construct.
The
items
that
remained
 were
examined
by
factor
using
both
statistical
strength
and
discussion
between
the
researchers.
 Statistical
strength
was
determined
based
on
a
comparison
of
the
means,
standard
deviation,
and
 standard
error
of
the
mean
for
each
item,
by
factor.
Items
were
ranked
based
on
strength
with
weaker
 items
being
eliminated.
If
there
were
no
items
that
were
clearly
weaker
based
on
statistical
strength,
 the
researchers
engaged
in
a
dialogue
in
which
they
described
the
factor
that
the
items
were
designed
 to
measure,
read
the
items
out
loud,
and
discussed
the
strengths
and
weaknesses
of
the
wording
of
the
 item.
If
necessary,
the
comments
given
by
those
who
pilot
tested
the
items
were
consulted
as
well.
 Discussion
occurred
until
an
agreement
on
moving
forward
with
or
without
the
item
was
made.
If
there
 were
truly
no
distinguishable
differences
based
on
strength
or
researcher
discussion
then
the
item
was
 kept
and
included
in
the
scale
reliability
analysis.
 The
scale
reliability
analysis
was
the
final
step
used
for
creating
each
scale.
The
items
that
had
been
 vetted
through
to
this
step
were
transferred
to
a
unique
database
in
SPSS
and
run
through
a
scale
 reliability
analysis.
The
results
of
the
reliability
analysis
were
examined
to
determine
the
level
of
 reliability
of
the
items
used
in
a
scale
(based
on
Cronbach’s
Alpha)
and
the
predicted
level
of
reliability
if
 any
of
the
item(s)
were
to
be
removed.
Items
that
increased
or
did
not
decrease
reliability
(based
on
 their
removal)
were
removed
if
other
items
in
the
scale
accounted
for
the
aspect
they
measured.
All
 final
scales
had
a
Cronbach’s
Alpha
of
at
least
.757;
exceeding
the
minimum
required
.700
for
use
in
 research.
 The
final
items
for
all
constructs
were
randomly
organized
into
the
final
single
scale.
Analysis
maintains
 each
item
representing
the
construct,
but
the
total
number
of
items
was
averaged
in
a
final
unified
scale
 to
represent
the
construct
including
aspects
that
were
removed
because
they
are
predicted
by
the
items
 remaining.
Confirmatory
factor
analysis,
reliability
comparisons
with
the
constructed
scale
reliability,
 and
classic
test
analysis
(Kappa
distance
measures)
all
confirmed
the
reliability
in
the
pilot
test
against
 the
final
survey
results.
 Following
reliability
and
validity
testing
of
the
instrument,
ILI
commissioned
an
online
consumer
survey
 (N=2,138)
from
a
commercial
vendor
to
determine
American
attitudes
toward
children
and
nature.
 Findings
from
this
survey
are
considered
reliable
with
a
2.1%
margin
of
error
for
the
American
 population.

American
Attitudes
Toward
Children’s
Nature
Experiences

15
































































June
2010


Findings
 Beliefs
in
the
Value
of
Childhood
Nature
Experiences
 The
survey
results
demonstrated
general
support
for
all
constructs
associated
with
encouraging
 children’s
nature
experiences
as
illustrated
in
Table
1.


 Table
1:
Summated
scale
descriptive
statistics.
Total
N=2138
 Scale

S.E.
 Mean

Scale
 SD

Healthiness

Scale
 
 5.86

Scale
 Reliability
 Pre

.023

1.08

.958

.971

Cognitive/Emotional
Growth

5.26

.025

1.13

.884

.938

Emotional
Well‐being

5.56

.024

1.11

.922

.956

Enhanced
Skills

5.65

.024

1.09

.898

.949

Appreciation
of
Nature

5.85

.023

1.09

.909

.956

Priority*

6.30

.025

1.15

.798

.941

Child
Safety*

4.90

.024

1.10

.860

.862

Storytelling

5.07

.031

1.42

.916

.922

Effort/Risk*

5.85

.024

1.09

.757

.833

Intentionality

6.11

.023

1.08

.905

.952

Scale

Reliability
 Post

Notes:


 (1)
Values
based
on
responses
to
a
7
pointLikert‐type
scale
with
1
representing
strongly
disagree
and
7
representing
strongly
agree.

 (2)*
Designates
scale
contains
items
that
were
reverse
coded.

Normative
Beliefs:
 These
data
demonstrate
that
Americans
generally
hold
normative
beliefs
that
experiences
in
nature
 help
childhood
development.
The
findings
from
the
C&NN
benchmarking
study
indicate
that
Americans
 believe
that
spending
time
in
nature
facilitates
children’s
appreciation
of
nature
and
that
these
 experiences
also
contribute
to
a
child’s
healthiness
including
physical
fitness,
happiness,
creativity,
and
 reduced
incidence
of
conditions
like
asthma
and
myopia
(mean
=
5.86).
To
only
a
slightly
lesser
extent,
 they
also
believe
that
children’s
outdoor
nature
experiences
support
increased
confidence,
self‐esteem,
 and
independence
defined
as
emotional
well‐being
(mean
=
5.56)
and
similarly
enhanced
skills
such
as
 learning
to
cooperate,
manage
stress
and
develop
stronger
relationships
(mean
=
5.65).
In
general,
they
 agree,
but
at
lower
levels
than
some
of
the
other
constructs,
that
spending
time
in
nature
facilitates
 children’s
cognitive
and
emotional
development
(mean
=
5.26).

 Control
Beliefs:
 Participants
in
this
survey
reflected
a
pronounced
agreement
that
spending
time
in
nature
should
be
a
 high
priority
for
encouraging
children’s
outdoor
activities,
after
normalizing
the
data
by
reverse
coding
 items
that
were
negatively
worded.
These
same
participants
also
expressed
a
slight
preference
for
the
 safety
concerns
that
might
limit
their
willingness
to
support
children’s
free‐play
in
nature.
That
is,
 participants
seemed
to
agree
that
safety
issues
play
a
role
in
their
decision
to
allow
children
to
play
 outdoors.
Data
for
the
safety
scale
were
also
normalized
by
reverse
coding
the
items
that
were
 negatively
worded
in
the
scale.

 Behavioral
Beliefs:
 The
findings
from
the
C&NN
benchmarking
study
reflect
a
preference
against
the
barriers
identified
by
 the
effort/risk
scale
that
assessed
the
perceived
relationship
between
encouraging
children’s
outdoor
 Institute for Learning Innovation

16

Results
of
the
2010
C&NN
EC‐NES
National
Survey


activity
and
the
potential
risk
these
children
might
face
when
in
nature.
That
is,
participants
do
not
 agree
with
the
concept
that
they
would
let
things
like
potential
injury
or
lack
of
modeling
behavior
by
 schools
prevent
them
from
supporting
children
spending
time
in
nature.
The
higher
mean
(mean
=
5.85)
 reflects
a
reverse
coded
result
and
demonstrates
stronger
amount
of
disagreement
with
the
items
 worded
as
barriers
on
the
survey.
But
again,
this
effort/risk
trade‐off
is
mitigated
by
their
beliefs
in
 modeling
behaviors
as
illustrated
by
their
near
neutral
to
slight
agreement
with
statements
related
to
 storytelling.
These
data
do
not
suggest
high
level
of
participant
engagement
with
storytelling
or
reading
 about
nature
and
animals,
or
that
participants
feel
it
is
important
for
children
to
hear
these
stories
 (mean
=
5.07).
 Intention:
 Finally,
these
participants’
responses
reflect
pronounced
preference
for
the
intention
to
support
 children’s
nature
contact
experiences.
Data
suggest
participants
intend
to
engage
in
the
activities
 identified
by
the
researchers
to
promote
children
having
access
to
nature
with
an
overall
mean
for
items
 of
6.11
with
a
standard
deviation
of
1.08.
However,
as
illustrated
in
Table
2
and
in
the
behavior
and
 control
beliefs,
concerns
for
safety
and
type
of
nature
place
have
a
direct
impact
on
whether
these
 adults
will
actually
support
these
behaviors
in
practice.


 Table
2:
Comparison
of
where
participants
were
allowed
to
play
as
children
and
where
they
would
 allow
their
children
to
play
(in
percent
of
total).
 When
you
were
a
child,
where
 were
you
allowed
to
play
 unsupervised?

Location

N=2138

In
which
of
these
places
would
you
 allow
your
child
age
7‐11
to
play
 unsupervised?

Total
Yes

Total
No

Total
Yes

Total
No

At
home
or
friend’s
home

89.9

10.1

84.2

15.8

Indoor
activity
area

75.0

25.0

75.6

24.4

School
playground

82.0

18.0

68.4

31.6

Indoor
after
school
club

55.4

44.6

72.1

27.9

Indoor
sports
center

48.6

51.4

62.2

37.8

In
the
streets
near
my
home

73.7

26.3

46.3

53.7

Garden

76.4

23.6

73.8

26.2

School
playing
fields

80.9

19.1

67.4

32.6

Outdoor
adventure
playground

63.3

36.7

58.6

41.4

Woods

66.5

33.5

40.5

59.5

Scrubland/fields/farmland

64.1

35.9

52.9

47.1

Riverside/creekside/pond

59.2

40.8

34.7

65.3

Mountains/grassy
hills/other
wild
places

63.0

37.0

45.7

54.3

Table
2
indicates
a
significant
variation
between
types
of
experiences
adults
may
have
had
as
children
 that
they
believed
were
permitted
by
their
parents
and
their
perception
of
these
same
places
as
safe
for
 children
today.
Woods,
creeks/rivers/ponds,
and
“wild
places”
are
considered
significantly
less
safe,
 whereas
these
same
adults
were
significantly
more
likely
to
want
children
to
play
in
indoor
places
even
 though
they
did
not
have
these
same
experiences
as
children.
These
data
suggest
that
the
type
of
 accessible
nature
is
considered
challenging
and
may
limit
willingness
to
support
children’s
opportunities
 to
explore
these
nature
places.
That
is,
if
adults
view
woods
or
creeks
as
unsafe
play
areas,
children
will
 not
be
allowed
to
play
in
these
areas
regardless
of
their
availability.

American
Attitudes
Toward
Children’s
Nature
Experiences

17
































































June
2010


Variations
in
Beliefs
by
Demographic
Variables
 The
greatest
variations
predicting
agreement
with
the
beliefs
were
participants’
sex,
followed
closely
by
 the
presence
of
a
child
in
the
household,
and
then
having
been
a
parent
or
guardian
at
some
point
in
life
 (Table
3).
Further
analysis
indicated
that
sex
was
the
primary
predictive
variable.
However,
all
three
of
 these
variables
are
related,
suggesting
that
the
support
for
childhood
nature
experiences
is
more
likely
 to
be
supported
by
mothers,
but
messages
targeting
parents
and
grandparents
will
most
likely
reach
a
 highly
receptive
audience.


 Table
3.
Summated
scale
 
by
gender,
parental
status
and
presence
of
a
child
in
the
home.

Sex

N

Parent
or
 guardian

Male

Female

Yes

No

Child
living
at
 home
 Yes

No

981

1148

1479

659

745

1393

Scale
































































%

45.9

53.7

69.2

30.8

34.8

65.2

Healthiness

5.73

5.97

5.95

5.66

5.87

5.85

Cognitive/Emotional
Growth

5.12

5.38

5.33

5.11

5.27

5.26

Emotional
Well‐being

5.47

5.63

5.64

5.38

5.53

5.57

Enhanced
Skills

5.51

5.77

5.72

5.49

5.63

5.66

Appreciation
of
Nature

5.67

6.01

4.99

4.70

5.86

5.85

Priority*

6.10

6.48

6.45

5.98

6.33

6.29

Child
Safety*

4.85

4.94

4.99

4.70

4.86

4.92

Storytelling

4.85

5.25

5.28

4.59

5.28

4.96

Effort/Risk*

5.64

6.03

6.00

5.51

5.92

5.82

Intentionality

5.94

6.26

6.25

5.80

6.16

6.09

Notes:


 (1)
Values
based
on
responses
to
a
7
point
Likert‐type
scale
with
1
representing
strongly
disagree
and
7
representing
strongly
agree.

 (2)*
Designates
scale
contains
items
that
were
reverse
coded.

Race/ethnicity
appeared
to
also
be
a
significant
predictor
of
belief
in
the
constructs
surrounding
the
 value
of
childhood
nature
experiences
(Table
4).
These
questions
were
structured
to
allow
participants
 to
select
all
affiliations
that
they
felt
applied.
Of
the
respondents,
112
indicated
Latino
heritage,
with
61
 choosing
not
to
select
a
racial
affiliation
while
the
rest
indicated
one
or
more
racial
heritage.
These
 results
demonstrated
that
racial
heritage
did
predict
beliefs.
For
those
with
Caucasian/White
heritage,
 there
was
a
preference
to
select
higher
agreement
ratings
with
the
values
and
belief
statements.
While
 the
Asian/Pacific
Islander
populations,
other,
and
Latino
communities
generally
had
lower
agreement
 with
the
value
of
childhood
nature
experiences
as
normative
beliefs,
there
was
still
general
agreement
 supporting
the
construct
with
the
notable
exception
of
concern
for
safety.
African
Americans
were
 notably
lower
in
their
agreement
about
the
value
of
nature
experiences
for
supporting
 cognitive/emotional
development
and
were
also
distinctly
concerned
with
their
children’s
safety.
It
was
 noted
that
American
Indians/First
Nations
were
more
likely
to
support
the
value
of
nature
for
helping
 children
across
all
constructs
and
noting
a
significantly
higher
degree
of
priority
for
these
experiences
in
 their
children’s
lives.
With
only
33
respondents,
these
data
suggest
a
pattern
but
caution
should
be
used
 in
this
interpretation
as
generalizable
across
all
First
Nations/American
Indian
peoples
because
this
 population
is
heterogeneous
in
their
beliefs
and
values.

Institute for Learning Innovation

18

Results
of
the
2010
C&NN
EC‐NES
National
Survey


Table
4:
Summated
scale
 
by
race/ethnicity1
 American
 Indian/First
 Nations

Asian/Pacific
 Islander

Black/African
 American

Other

Spanish,
 Hispanic
 Latino

White/
 Caucasian

N

33

71

112

61

112

1860

Scale

























%

1.5

3.3

5.2

2.9

5.2

87.0

Healthiness

5.89

5.86

5.64

5.63

5.66

5.89

Cognitive/Emotional
Growth

5.07

5.26

4.98

5.31

5.00

5.29

Emotional
Well‐being

5.54

5.64

5.43

5.27

5.29

5.58

Enhanced
Skills

5.61

5.63

5.51

5.50

5.44

5.68

Appreciation
of
Nature

5.91

5.86

5.69

5.56

5.86

5.88

Priority*

6.46

5.68

5.95

6.16

6.16

6.36

Child
Safety*

5.05

4.27

5.69

4.60

4.54

4.96

Storytelling

5.38

4.93

4.99

4.87

4.89

5.09

Effort/Risk*

5.91

5.23

4.42

5.67

5.72

5.90

Intentionality

6.15

6.02

5.96

5.83

5.97

6.15

Notes:


 (1)
Values
based
on
responses
to
a
7
point
Likert‐type
scale
with
1
representing
strongly
disagree
and
7
representing
strongly
agree.

 (2)*
Designates
scale
contains
items
that
were
reverse
coded.

To
further
explore
demographic
variation
in
beliefs,
we
examined
whether
race
and
cultural
heritage
 had
any
relationship
to
nature
play
opportunities
offered
to
adults
when
they
were
children
and
 whether
these
experiences
translated
into
a
higher
likelihood
to
allow
children
the
opportunity
for
free
 exploration
of
nature
today.
The
results
(Table
5)
demonstrated
that
race
and
cultural
heritage
were
 predictive
of
access
to
all
types
of
nature
play
places
with
White/Caucasian
and
American
Indian/First
 Nations
people
having
more
access
than
other
race/cultural
groups.
The
percentage
willing
to
allow
 children
these
opportunities
today,
however,
did
not
vary.
Of
all
those
who
had
personal
opportunities
 for
nature
play
as
children
in
wooded
areas,
near
creeks
and
streams,
or
mountains
and
wild
places,
 only
62%
would
allow
their
children
the
same
opportunities
today.
On
all
other
criteria,
adults
were
as
 likely
to
allow
children
to
have
the
same
access.
Asian
and
Pacific
Islander
adults
in
this
sample
were
 more
likely
to
allow
children
to
have
the
types
of
experiences
that
they
had
as
children
than
all
other
 groups,
but
the
magnitude
of
this
difference
was
small.

1

It
is
important
to
note
that
the
response
rates
for
minority
populations
represented
in
this
survey
have
a
lower
 predictive
strength
than
the
total
American
population.

As
noted
above,
the
American
Indian/First
Nations
 results
can
only
be
considered
anecdotal
but
highlight
the
need
for
more
research
to
determine
accurate
beliefs
 for
these
peoples.

Similarly,
while
the
other
minority
communities
had
greater
representation
in
the
survey
data
 and
can
be
inferred
to
broadly
represent
their
communities,
the
margin
of
error
for
these
data
would
 individually
be
considered
accurate
with
a
margin
of
error
of
plus
or
minus
9.26%
at
the
95%
level
of
confidence,
 and
for
non‐white/Caucasian
minorities
in
general
excluding
American
Indians/First
Nations,
within
plus
or
 minus
5.19%
at
the
95%
level
of
confidence.

American
Attitudes
Toward
Children’s
Nature
Experiences

19
































































June
2010


Table
5:
Where
adults
played
and
where
they
would
allow
children
to
play
by
race

American
 Indian/
 First
 Nations

Asian/
 Pacific
 Islander

Black/
 African
 American

Other

Spanish
 Hispanic
 Latino

White/
 Caucasian

N

33

71

112

61

112

1860

Location

%

1.5

3.3

5.2

2.9

5.2

87.0

Allowed
as
child

90.9

93.0

84.8

86.9

87.5

90.3

Would
allow
child

84.8

91.5

82.1

83.6

84.4

84.4

Allowed
as
child

75.8

78.9

82.1

80.3

78.6

74.5

Would
allow
child

78.8

73.2

73.2

82.0

83.0

75.6

Allowed
as
child

75.8

85.9

84.8

73.8

82.1

82.0

Would
allow
child

66.7

78.9

75.0

62.3

67.9

67.8

Allowed
as
child

54.5

71.8

63.4

60.7

55.1

54.1

Would
allow
child

72.7

78.9

78.6

73.8

67.0

71.8

Allowed
as
child

48.5

56.3

58.9

55.7

57.1

47.7

Would
allow
child

60.6

66.2

71.4

62.3

66.1

61.4

Allowed
as
child

72.7

76.1

63.4

72.1

73.2

74.5

Would
allow
child

45.5

53.5

42.9

47.5

47.3

46.2

Allowed
as
child

78.8

71.8

65.2

67.2

73.2

78.0

Would
allow
child

81.8

80.3

64.3

62.3

66.1

75.0

Allowed
as
child

81.8

84.5

80.4

73.8

75.9

80.9

Would
allow
child

66.7

69.0

71.4

60.7

63.4

67.1

Allowed
as
child

75.8

59.2

67.9

60.7

70.5

63.2

Would
allow
child

54.5

62.0

64.3

47.5

53.6

58.4

Allowed
as
child

69.7

42.3

42.9

45.9

48.2

69.7

Would
allow
child

39.4

25.4

20.5

27.9

24.1

42.8

Allowed
as
child

69.7

39.4

41.1

49.2

50.9

67.1

Would
allow
child

57.6

39.4

33.0

37.7

39.3

55.5

Allowed
as
child

54.5

40.8

43.8

47.5

43.8

61.5

Would
allow
child

33.3

26.8

25.0

23.0

20.5

36.0

Allowed
as
child

60.6

45.1

43.8

44.3

50.0

65.7

Would
allow
child

48.5

33.8

20.4

32.8

33.0

47.7

At
home
or
friend’s
home
 Indoor
activity
area
 School
playground
 Indoor
after
school
club
 Indoor
sports
center
 In
the
streets
near
my
home
 Garden
 School
playing
fields
 Outdoor
adventure
 playground
 Woods
 Shrubland/fields/farmland
 Riverside/creekside/pond
 Mountains/grassy
hills/other
 wild
places

Note:
Value
based
on
percent
of
total.

Surprisingly,
Americans
were
very
consistent
in
their
agreement
with
the
belief
statements
whether
 they
lived
in
cities,
suburbs
or
towns
over
5,000
people
(Table
6).

However,
those
living
in
very
small
 towns
less
than
5,000
people
and
those
living
in
rural
areas
were
more
willing
to
strongly
agree
with
the
 beliefs
statements
related
to
nature
experiences
at
all
levels.
While
these
data
are
significant
and
 indicate
a
weighted
opinion,
the
data
indicate
that
this
agreement
is
a
matter
of
degree
rather
than
 conflicting
beliefs.

Institute for Learning Innovation

20

Results
of
the
2010
C&NN
EC‐NES
National
Survey


Table
6:
Summated
scale
 
by
location
type
of
current
residence

N

Large
 city
 250,000+

City
 100,000‐ 249,000

City
 50,000‐ 99,999

Small
city
 25,000‐
 49,999

Town
 10,000‐ 24,999

Town
 5,000‐ 9,999

Town
 under
 5,000

Farm/
 rural
 area

153

178

214

457

310

258

309

259

Scale























%

21.4

14.5

12.1

14.5

12.1

7.2

8.3

10.0

Healthiness

5.89

5.79

5.75

5.84

5.80

5.75

6.02

6.08

Cognitive/Emotional
Growth

5.26

5.18

5.18

5.21

5.25

5.26

5.32

5.51

Emotional
Well‐being

5.57

5.46

5.47

5.57

5.48

5.53

5.69

5.78

Enhanced
Skills

5.66

5.61

5.50

5.64

5.60

5.58

5.76

5.88

Appreciation
of
Nature

5.87

5.78

5.67

5.84

5.84

5.81

6.01

6.07

Priority*

6.26

6.23

6.20

6.22

6.39

6.42

6.42

6.46

Child
Safety*

4.82

4.84

4.82

4.88

4.91

4.85

5.10

5.14

Storytelling

5.04

4.95

4.91

5.01

5.11

5.16

5.24

5.31

Effort/Risk*

5.84

5.80

5.69

5.81

5.88

5.88

6.03

6.02

Intentionality

6.12

6.03

5.94

6.07

6.11

6.16

6.29

6.31

Notes:


 (1)
Values
based
on
responses
to
a
7
point
Likert‐type
scale
with
1
representing
strongly
disagree
and
7
representing
strongly
agree.

 (2)*
Designates
scale
contains
items
that
were
reverse
coded.

We
further
explored
whether
location
of
residence
also
predicted
willingness
to
allow
children
to
have
 the
opportunity
to
explore
nature
places
that
adults
had
when
they
were
children.
Unlike
the
 race/ethnicity
variables
found
to
have
a
direct
relationship
to
willingness
to
allow
access,
place
of
 residence
did
not
predict
any
significant
variation
in
the
data
(Appendix
B,
Table
B.8).
 Location
of
residence,
however,
is
a
statistical
fact,
whereas
the
perception
of
how
close
nature
might
 be
may
not
correlate
to
the
actual
size
of
a
town.
For
example,
urban
New
York
has
nearly
25%
of
the
 city
devoted
to
public
open‐space
and
large
parks
in
the
center
of
town,
while
some
small
towns
do
not
 have
any
public
open‐space.
When
asked
“how
far
nature
was
from
home,”
the
results
were
still
 somewhat
surprising
(Table
7).

16%
of
respondents
felt
that
the
closest
nature
place
suitable
for
 children’s
play
was
at
least
30
minutes
from
their
home,
while
40%
believed
that
nature
was
at
least
15
 minutes
from
their
home.
These
data
did
not
correlate
with
the
data
regarding
size
of
town,
income
or
 any
other
demographic
variable.
This
state
of
mind
regarding
the
proximity
to
acceptable
nature,
or
the
 “nature
of
nature,”
did
emerge
as
an
important
predictor
for
general
beliefs.
It
was
found
that
those
 who
perceive
nature
to
be
just
outside
their
door
are
more
likely
to
agree
more
strongly
with
the
value
 of
nature
experiences
than
those
who
believe
that
nature
is
distant
from
where
they
live.

It
is
important
 to
note
that
perception
may
not
necessarily
be
factually
true
but
it
is
important
to
note
that
some
urban
 areas
are,
in
fact,
quite
distant
from
planted
spaces
or
green
areas
that
allow
children
access.
Therefore,
 these
results
may
be
a
combination
of
perception
and
real
access
acting
in
concert
to
produce
a
feeling
 of
isolation
from
nature
that
translates
into
lower
beliefs
that
nature
is
of
value
to
children.

American
Attitudes
Toward
Children’s
Nature
Experiences

21
































































June
2010


Table
7:
Summated
scale
 
by
how
far
are
you
from
nature

Just
 outside

5
minute
 walk

15
 minute
 walk

15
 minutes
 by
car

30
 minutes
 by
car

30
 minutes
 by
bus

1
 hour

90+
 minutes

N

569

355

320

544

215

11

66

58

Scale



%

26.6

16.6

15.0

25.4

10.1

0.5

3.1

2.7

Healthiness

6.09

5.83

5.78

5.85

5.77

6.11

5.61

4.91

Cognitive/Emotional
Growth

5.47

5.21

5.23

5.23

5.16

5.31

5.09

4.54

Emotional
Well‐being

5.77

5.55

5.51

5.51

5.45

5.72

5.46

4.78

Enhanced
Skills

5.91

5.64

5.57

5.59

5.53

5.74

5.46

4.81

Appreciation
of
Nature

6.08

5.81

5.80

5.83

5.76

5.97

5.63

4.89

Priority*

6.49

6.25

6.19

6.32

6.36

6.11

6.01

5.36

Child
Safety*

5.17

4.95

4.82

4.85

4.70

3.91

4.56

4.21

Storytelling

5.36

5.03

5.08

5.03

4.87

5.36

4.50

4.06

Effort/Risk*

6.04

5.84

5.76

5.85

5.86

5.33

5.53

5.05

Intentionality

6.33

6.10

6.02

6.13

6.04

6.29

5.80

5.09

Notes:


 (1)
Values
based
on
responses
to
a
7
pointLikert‐type
scale
with
1
representing
strongly
disagree
and
7
representing
strongly
agree.

 (2)*
Designates
scale
contains
items
that
were
reverse
coded.

Interestingly,
the
perception
of
how
close
nature
is
to
home
was
found
to
be
predictive
of
beliefs
and
 also
related
to
experiences
these
adults
had
as
children.
It
appeared
that
those
participants
who
felt
 that
nature
was
further
away
from
their
home
were
less
likely
to
report
that
they
were
allowed
to
play
 independently
as
children
in
nature
and
wild
places,
and,
in
some
cases,
any
place
at
all
where
they
 were
allowed
free
play.
While
these
experiences
were
no
more
likely
to
predict
their
willingness
to
allow
 children
opportunities
for
free
play,
it
did
indicate
that
they
may
not
have
a
point
of
reference
for
those
 experiences
(Table
8).

Institute for Learning Innovation

22

Results
of
the
2010
C&NN
EC‐NES
National
Survey


Table
8:
What
were
you
allowed
to
do
and
what
would
you
allow
a
child
to
do
by
how
far
from
nature
 Just
 outside

5
min
 walk

N

569

355

320

544

215

Location

%

26.6

16.6

15.0

25.4

10.1

0.5

3.1

2.7

At
home
or
friend’s
 home

Allowed
as
child

91.6

91.5

88.4

91.7

86.0

90.9

86.4

70.7

Would
allow
child

87.3

84.8

85.6

83.6

83.3

72.7

78.8

58.6

Allowed
as
child

72.4

77.7

76.6

76.3

74.4

72.7

80.3

60.3

Would
allow
child

76.8

75.5

75.0

74.4

78.1

81.8

78.8

65.5

Allowed
as
child

79.6

81.7

84.4

85.5

80.5

90.9

83.3

65.5

Would
allow
child

67.0

73.8

70.9

65.6

68.8

54.5

72.7

58.6

Allowed
as
child

51.0

60.3

60.3

55.7

54.9

54.5

51.5

44.8

Would
allow
child

69.9

76.1

74.7

71.7

73.5

63.6

68.2

58.6

Allowed
as
child

45.2

52.7

53.8

48.7

44.7

63.6

50.0

37.9

Would
allow
child

61.0

64.5

67.5

59.4

63.3

72.7

60.6

51.7

Allowed
as
child

71.4

77.7

75.0

74.4

75.3

63.6

75.8

51.7

Would
allow
child

46.4

53.0

47.8

43.0

44.7

45.5

48.5

31.0

Allowed
as
child

80.7

76.1

81.3

74.1

73.0

72.7

72.7

50.0

Would
allow
child

80.0

76.1

76.9

69.9

67.0

72.7

69.7

50.0

Allowed
as
child

78.7

83.1

83.8

82.9

78.1

81.8

77.3

67.2

Would
allow
child

67.5

71.3

68.8

64.9

69.3

54.5

66.7

53.4

Allowed
as
child

61.5

70.1

66.9

64.0

59.1

45.5

51.5

46.6

Would
allow
child

59.9

59.7

62.8

55.3

57.7

63.6

62.1

43.1

Allowed
as
child

75.4

67.6

64.1

66.7

60.9

45.5

47.0

31.0

Would
allow
child

54.1

39.7

36.6

35.8

31.2

27.3

34.8

20.7

Shrubland/fields/

Allowed
as
child

72.6

69.0

62.8

60.1

59.1

36.4

45.5

41.4

farmland

Would
allow
child

66.1

52.4

52.5

46.5

47.4

18.2

39.4

32.8

Riverside/
 creekside/pond

Allowed
as
child

65.9

60.3

59.4

58.8

54.0

36.4

39.4

36.2

Would
allow
child

40.1

38.9

32.8

31.6

26.5

27.3

36.4

25.9

Mountains/
grassy
hills/
 other
wild
places

Allowed
as
child

70.5

65.6

64.4

60.7

54.9

54.5

43.9

41.4

Would
allow
child

57.1

46.2

43.4

39.9

37.7

54.5

47.0

25.9

Indoor
activity
area
 School
playground
 Indoor
after
school
club
 Indoor
sports
center
 In
the
streets
near
my
 home
 Garden
 School
playing
fields
 Outdoor
adventure
 playground
 Woods

15
min
 15
min
 30
min
 30
min
 walk
 by
car
 by
car
 by
bus
 11

1
 hour

90+
 min

66

58

Secondary
Factor
Analysis
 The
summated
scales
were
put
through
a
secondary
Exploratory
Factor
Analysis
to
determine
whether
 the
subscales
were
consistent
with
the
Theory
of
Planned
Behavior
structure,
and
to
determine
if
the
 variance
within
the
dataset
could
be
explained.
This
analysis
revealed
two
clear
factors
with
one
item
 (scale)
crossing
the
two
discrete
factors
(Table
9).
The
first
factor
is
comprised
of
the
Normative
Beliefs
 scales
(Healthiness,
Cognitive/Emotional
Growth,
Emotional
Well‐being,
Enhanced
Skills,
and
 Appreciation
of
Nature),
the
Storytelling
scale,
and
the
Intentionality
scale.
The
second
factor
was
 comprised
of
the
Control
scales
(Priority
and
Child
Safety)
and
the
Effort/Risk
scale.
These
relationships
 appeared
both
logical
and
appropriate
since
the
child‐centric
Normative
Belief
Scales
support
 intentionality
as
would
the
positive
mentoring
behaviors
associated
with
storytelling
behavioral
beliefs.
 The
control
beliefs
and
mitigating
behavioral
belief
factor
of
adult
effort/risk
represent
potential
barriers
 to
the
behaviors
being
studied
that
would
support
encouraging
children
to
spend
time
in
nature.
 American
Attitudes
Toward
Children’s
Nature
Experiences

23
































































June
2010


Table
9:
Factor
loadings
from
principal
component
analysis

Component
Loading
Scores

Factor

Component
1

Component
2

Healthiness

.897

Cognitive/Emotional
Growth

.761

Emotional
Well‐being

.882

Enhanced
Skills

.898

Appreciation
of
Nature

.905

Priority*

.626

.639

.741

Child
Safety*
 Storytelling

.648

Effort/Risk*

.706

.853

Intentionality

Notes:


 (1)
Values
based
on
responses
to
a
7
pointLikert‐type
scale
with
1
representing
strongly
disagree
and
7
representing
strongly
agree.

 (2)*
Designates
scale
contains
items
that
were
reverse
coded.

Following
identification
of
these
discrete
factors,
regression
analysis
against
proximity
to
nature
 revealed
that
both
combined
factors
were
significant,
and
the
child‐centric
scale
(component
1)
 remained
negligible
in
amount
of
variance
explained
(Tables
10
and
11)
with
4.0%
variance
explained
for
 the
child‐centric
scales
but
a
slightly
higher
17.5%
variance
explained
by
the
parental
control
scales.
This
 would
suggest
that
proximity
to
nature
and
parental
control
factors
have
a
linear
relationship
and
that
 adults’
perception
of
the
importance
of
being
in
nature
and
their
proximity
to
nature
has
a
weak
but
 significant
predictive
relationship.
Logically,
beliefs
and
values
about
child
development
and
nature
have
 a
negligible
but
consistent
(statistically
significant)
predictive
relationship.
 Table
10:
Results
of
regression
analysis
on
proximity
to
nature
predicting
Normative
Beliefs
Scales,
 Storytelling,
and
Intentionality
 Type
III
Sum
of
 Squares

df

Mean
Square

F

Sig

86.335ª

7

12.334

13.59

.000

14085.741

1

14085.741

15531.275

.000

86.335

7

12.334

13.599

.000

Error

1931.756

2130

.907

Total

69618.388

2138

Corrected
Total

2018.090

2137

Source
 Corrected
Model
 Intercept
 Proximity
to
nature

a.
R
Squared=.043
(Adjusted
R
Squared=.040)

Institute for Learning Innovation

24

Results
of
the
2010
C&NN
EC‐NES
National
Survey


Table
11:
Results
of
Regression
Analysis
for
Proximity
to
Nature,
Normative
Beliefs,
Storytelling
and
 Intentionality
on
Priority,
Child
Safety,
Effort/Risk
 Source

Type
III
Sum
of
 Squares

df

Corrected

Model

358.508ª

8

Intercept

642.867

1

Normative
Beliefs/
 Storytelling/Intentionality

280.789

1

Proximity
to
nature

29.964

Mean
Square

F

Sig

44.814

57.644

.000

642.867

826.927

.000

280.789

361.183

.000

4.281

5.506

.000

Error

1655.121

7

.777

Total

71123.372

2129

Corrected
Total

2013.629

2138

a.
R
Squared=.178
(Adjusted
R
Squared=.175)

Assessment
of
Age,
Income,
Education
and
State
of
Residence
 As
expected,
income
and
education
had
a
minor
but
significant
impact
on
beliefs
with
those
in
 increasingly
higher
income
brackets
or
having
higher
levels
of
academic
achievement
being
more
likely
 to
support
all
beliefs
and
intentions.
These
variations
(as
illustrated
in
Appendix
B,
Tables
B.7
and
B.8)
 were
not
substantial
in
magnitude
even
though
there
is
a
clear
pattern.
 Not
surprisingly,
age
had
an
impact
on
beliefs,
with
younger
participants
aged
18
–
33
years
agreeing
 less
strongly
and
agreement
increasing
consistently
through
to
participants
aged
65‐73
(Table
12
and
 Table
13).

 Table
12:
Values
by
Generation

Age
Group
 18‐33
Years

34‐45
Years

46‐55
Years

56‐64
Years

65‐73
Years

74+
Years

N

470

604

432

253

180

199

Scale
























%

22

28.3

20.2

11.8

8.4

9.3

Healthiness

5.59

6.33

6.46

6.5

6.61

6.41

Cognitive/Emotional
Growth

5.04

5.25

5.3

5.38

5.55

5.31

Emotional
Well‐being

5.35

5.55

5.57

5.69

5.81

5.66

Enhanced
Skills

5.44

5.66

5.64

5.73

5.91

5.79

Appreciation
of
Nature

5.64

5.8

5.92

5.96

6.13

5.98

Priority*

5.86

6.33

6.46

6.5

6.61

6.41

Child
Safety*

4.62

4.85

5.05

5.08

5.1

4.99

Storytelling

4.96

5.01

5.13

5.08

5.22

5.2

Effort/Risk*

5.54

5.83

5.98

6.05

6.11

5.9

Intentionality

5.81

6.11

6.18

6.23

6.38

6.29

Notes:


 (1)
Values
based
on
responses
to
a
7
pointLikert‐type
scale
with
1
representing
strongly
disagree
and
7
representing
strongly
agree.

 (2)*
Designates
scale
contains
items
that
were
reverse
coded.

 (3)With
the
exception
of
the
storytelling
scale,
all
scales
were
significantly
different
between
age
groups
at
p=.000

American
Attitudes
Toward
Children’s
Nature
Experiences

25
































































June
2010


Table
13:

Age
Distribution
in
Survey
and
Summary
Response
 Generation
Name

Birth
Years

Age
in
2009

American
 Population

Americans
 Online

Survey
 Participants

N

Summated
 Response

Gen
Y
(Millennials)

Born
1977‐1990

Ages
18‐32

26%

30%

22.0%

470

5.39

Gen
X
Born

Born
1965‐1976

Ages
33‐44

20%

23%

28.3%

604

5.67

Younger
Boomers

Born
1955‐1964

Ages
45‐54

20%

22%

20.2%

432

5.77

Older
Boomers

Born
1946‐1954

Ages
55‐63

13%

13%

11.8%

253

5.82

Silent
Generation

Born
1937‐1945

Ages
64‐72

9%

7%

8.4%

180

5.94

G.I.
Generation*

Born
1936
or
earlier

Age
73+

9%

4%

9.3%

199

5.79

Source:
Pew
Internet
&
American
Life
Project
December
2008
survey.
N=2,253
total
adults,
margin
of
error
is
±2%.
N=1,650

These
data
appear
to
align
with
Erik
Erikson’s
research
on
social
concerns
through
the
life‐cycle.2
 Erikson’s
research
showed
that
greater
focus
on
others
and
leaving
a
healthier
society
are
more
 pronounced
with
age.
These
factors
did
not
correlate
to
any
other
distinction
in
the
data.
 The
survey
did
explore
whether
choice
of
news
station
had
an
impact
on
beliefs
(Table
B.6).
While
these
 data
did
show
variation
beyond
random
chance
due
to
the
large
sample
size,
the
magnitude
of
that
 variation
was
insignificant
and
did
not
reflect
any
substantial
reason
to
believe
that
any
particular
 political
viewpoint
had
any
lower
or
higher
belief
in
the
value
of
nature
experiences
for
children.
 We
also
explored
whether
state
of
residence
had
an
impact
on
beliefs
and
intentions
and
discovered
a
 few
notable
variations
(Summary
Table
14).
California
residents
were
less
likely
to
tell
stories
about
their
 nature
experiences
than
other
Americans,
while
Colorado
residents
tended
to
agree
more
strongly
with
 the
priority
for
encouraging
nature
experiences,
believe
their
efforts
outweighed
the
risks
to
children,
 and
more
strongly
agreed
that
children
develop
enhanced
skills,
healthiness,
and
emotional
well‐being
 from
having
nature
experiences.
Florida
residents,
on
the
other
hand,
were
more
inhibited
in
their
 willingness
to
allow
children
to
explore
nature
experiences
because
of
safety
concerns
while
Michigan
 residents
were
the
opposite
and
less
concerned
about
safety.
Results
from
New
Hampshire,
Texas,
Ohio
 and
New
Mexico
were
not
significantly
different
from
the
national
data.

2

Erikson,
E.
H.
(1985).
Childhood
and
Society.
(3rd
ed.).
New
York:
W.
W.
Norton.
 Erikson,
E.
H.,
&
Erikson,
J.
M.
(1997).
The
Life
Cycle
Completed.
New
York:
W.
W.
Norton
&
Company
Inc.

Institute for Learning Innovation

26

Results
of
the
2010
C&NN
EC‐NES
National
Survey


Table
13:
Summated
Scale
 
by
selected
state
 CO

Total
 Scale

CA
 Scale

Scale
































N

2138

164

Scale
 
 79

Healthiness

5.86

5.78

Cognitive/Emotional
Growth

5.26

5.29

Emotional
Well‐being

5.56

Enhanced
Skills

5.65

Appreciation
of
Nature
 Priority

NH

FL
 Scale

MI
 Scale

109

125

88

6.22

5.81

5.81

5.51

5.29

5.17

5.61

5.82

5.50

5.61

5.86

5.58

5.85

5.85

6.08

6.30

6.28

6.57

Child
Safety*

4.90

4.80

Storytelling

5.07

Effort/Risk*

5.85

Intentionality

6.11

TX

NM
 Scale

OH
 Scale

85

148

162

5.85

5.82

5.77

5.86

5.30

5.05

5.18

5.28

5.42

5.61

5.50

5.45

5.60

5.67

5.70

5.57

5.62

5.69

5.86

5.88

5.85

5.67

5.83

5.87

6.10

6.31

6.23

6.34

6.35

6.36

5.25

4.66

5.11

5.00

5.01

4.86

4.91

4.78

5.24

4.98

5.09

5.23

4.85

5.11

5.16

5.79

6.08

5.79

5.88

5.82

5.94

5.83

5.90

6.08

6.31

6.01

6.19

6.00

6.05

6.05

6.16

Scale

Scale

Notes:


 (1)
Values
based
on
responses
to
a
7
pointLikert‐type
scale
with
1
representing
strongly
disagree
and
7
representing
strongly
agree.

 (2)*
Designates
scale
contains
items
that
were
reverse
coded.

American
Attitudes
Toward
Children’s
Nature
Experiences

27
































































June
2010


Discussion
and
Recommendations
 These
data
illustrate
that
Americans
believe
nature
experiences
are
an
important
part
of
childhood
 development
and
generally
claim
that
they
intend
to
encourage
children
to
have
these
experiences.
As
 expected,
these
beliefs
tended
to
receive
stronger
support
from
women
than
men
and
from
parents
 than
those
who
have
never
had
children.
And
perhaps
not
surprisingly,
those
with
higher
levels
of
 academic
accomplishment
support
these
beliefs
more
strongly.
Concern
for
children’s
safety
in
nature
 places
and
the
perception
that
nature
is
not
near
to
one’s
home
are
two
barriers
that
predict
lower
 levels
of
intention
to
help
children
experience
nature.
It
was
also
revealed
that
beliefs
about
the
value
of
 nature
experiences
for
childhood
development
appear
to
be
weakening
with
each
succeeding
 generation.
Adults
who
were
not
allowed
to
have
nature
experiences
when
they
were
children
were
less
 likely
to
support
beliefs
that
nature
is
an
important
part
of
childhood
and
were
also
less
likely
to
allow
 children
these
opportunities
to
play
in
more
natural
areas
other
than
public
parks.
Variation
in
 responses
based
on
ethnicity
suggests
that
family
heritage
and
culture
may
have
a
direct
impact
on
 these
beliefs.

 On
the
surface,
these
results
appear
to
suggest
that
adults
support
beliefs
related
to
encouraging
 children
to
have
nature
experiences,
but
there
are
patterns
emerging
in
these
data
that
are
worthy
of
 concern.
The
following
section
discusses
possible
factors
that
may
have
influenced
these
results,
action
 steps
for
those
who
seek
to
better
understand
these
relationships,
and
recommendations
that
may
help
 to
overcome
these
perceived
barriers
to
encouraging
children’s
nature
experiences.
 Normative
Beliefs:
 In
general,
the
concept
of
nature
experiences
helping
children
develop
was
well
supported,
but
the
 support
was
associated
more
closely
with
physical
skills
improvement,
coordination
and
fitness
and
the
 appreciation
of
nature
than
the
more
emotional
and
cognitive
benefits
that
children
accrue.
While
the
 magnitude
of
this
difference
was
relatively
low,
this
difference
should
not
be
discounted
because
these
 areas
of
development
are
of
particular
concern
for
children
who
live
in
at‐risk
communities.

 Recommendations:
 1. 2.

In
general,
focusing
on
the
cognitive
and
emotional
benefits
of
outdoor
nature
for
cognitive
 development
and
increased
opportunity
for
success
in
formal
education
could
help
to
 expand
the
overall
beliefs
in
the
value
of
nature.



 We
note
that
these
dimensions
of
the
normative
beliefs
scales
were
consistently
lower
 whenever
demographic
variables
also
predicted
lower
scores.
Targeting
public
information
 campaigns
about
the
benefits
of
nature
experiences
toward
communities
and
groups
that
 have
lower
than
average
educational
achievement
and
economic
levels,
young
adults
and
 minority
communities
may
help
to
redress
appreciation
of
these
values
as
part
of
the
overall
 American
discourse.

Control
Beliefs:
 The
responses
to
the
control
belief
dimensions
of
the
survey
indicate
that
there
is
a
strong
emphasis
for
 adults
to
encourage
children
to
spend
time
outdoors,
but
this
priority
is
challenged
by
their
concerns
for
 child
safety.
The
responses
to
the
questions
about
where
adults
were
allowed
to
play
as
children
further
 demonstrated
a
possible
reason
for
that
trepidation.
These
data
suggest
that
near
two‐thirds
of
 American
adults
feel
they
were
allowed
to
play
in
wooded
areas,
near
streams,
or
other
relatively
 “natural”
or
wild
areas,
but
less
than
half
were
willing
to
allow
their
children
these
same
experiences.

 Institute for Learning Innovation

28

Results
of
the
2010
C&NN
EC‐NES
National
Survey


Furthermore,
those
who
did
not
have
those
experiences
were
even
more
likely
to
have
a
higher
 resistance
to
allowing
children
to
play
in
those
areas.
It
would
seem
that
nature
play
is
no
longer
a
 tradition
in
America.

Given
that
these
results
suggested
that
American
adults
are
more
likely
to
 encourage
the
children
in
their
lives
to
play
in
organized
play
spaces,
indoors
and
in
gardens,
there
is
an
 even
higher
likelihood
that
nature
experiences
will
continue
to
diminish
over
time.

 Recommendations:

 1.

2. 3. 4.

5.

Creating
opportunities
for
adults
to
learn
how
to
facilitate
children’s
nature
experiences
in
 more
wild
places,
by
discussing
practical
pre‐arming
strategies
to
help
them
mentor
their
 children
in
safe
play,
may
increase
the
likelihood
that
adults
are
more
willing
to
trust
their
 children.

 Assessing
reports
on
the
danger
in
the
woods
from
“bad
people”
or
other
possible
 concealed
threats
in
nature
places
to
report
practically
on
the
risk
children
will
face
if
they
 are
unaccompanied
may
demystify
the
sense
of
risk.
 Promoting
safe
and
accessible
“wild
places
for
kids”
may
increase
the
appeal
of
these
places
 to
children
and
help
shape
a
new
normative
belief
about
these
places
that
will
reduce
 parents’
control
beliefs
that
limit
their
willingness
to
allow
children
to
play
in
nature
places.

 Focusing
messaging
strategies
about
nature
play
using
terms
like
natural
and
park

rather
 than
the
loaded
terms
of
streams,
woods
or
wild
places,
may
seem
less
threatening
and
 increase
the
likelihood
that
adults
will
lower
their
concern
about
safety
risks
outweighing
 the
benefits
that
will
accrue
to
children.
 Establishing
campaigns
to
help
garden,
park
and
school
yard
managers
to
create
more
 “wild”
nature
experience
places
in
supervised
areas
may
help
to
create
bridge
experiences
 that
expose
children
to
nature
and
increase
the
likelihood
that
they
will
in
turn
be
more
 likely
to
encourage
their
own
children
to
gain
more
experiences
in
nature.

Behavioral
Beliefs:
 Perhaps
the
most
interesting
results
were
the
responses
to
the
behavioral
belief
statements.
These
 results
suggest
that
adults
do
try
to
encourage
children
to
experience
nature
in
spite
of
the
potential
 risks
they
face
or
the
safety
concerns
that
adults
have.
However,
adults
are
not
very
likely
to
tell
stories
 about
their
experiences
in
nature
or
about
nature
in
general.
The
survey
focused
on
storytelling
because
 it
has
been
suggested
that
parents’
narratives
about
their
own
childhood
shape
the
world
and
norms
in
 a
child’s
life.
The
research
on
which
this
aspect
of
the
scale
was
based
derives
primarily
from
cultural
 anthropology
and
environmental
education
rather
than
developmental
psychology.
We
speculate
that
 storytelling
is
an
indicator
for
types
of
mentoring
activities
that
adults
use
to
engage
children
in
their
 social
world.
From
a
developmental
psychology
perspective,
it
is
well
established
that
fairytales,
 television,
and
stories
from
family
members
help
shape
a
child’s
understanding
of
the
hierarchy,
 complexity
and
social
norms
that
will
shape
their
futures.
Again,
given
that
most
adults
were
not
 allowed
to
have
solitary
nature
experiences
when
they
were
children,
they
are
less
likely
to
encourage
 such
exploration
in
their
children,
and
if
they
don’t
create
these
stories,
the
separation
of
nature
from
 society
can
only
continue
to
grow.
Helping
adults
to
understand
the
importance
of
sharing
positive
 personal
stories
about
nature
may
help
stem
the
growing
sense
of
separation
people
have
from
nature.
 Recommendations:
 1.
 Creating
materials
may
help
adults
recall,
structure
and
tell
stories
to
children
about
their
 experiences
in
nature
may
help
interested
adults
accomplish
this
simple
task.
 2.
 Joining
with
reading,
writing
and
literacy
programs
to
highlight
adults’
personal
experiences
 in
nature
may
help
children
to
draw
these
stories
out
in
their
parents.
An
example
might
be
 a
writing
exercise
where
children
ask
their
parents
to
relate
a
story
about
their
time
in
 nature
as
children,
and
then
write
that
story
together.
 American
Attitudes
Toward
Children’s
Nature
Experiences

29
































































June
2010


3.
 Promoting
further
research
into
the
psychological
value
of
adult
storytelling
about
nature
 may
help
advance
knowledge
for
advocates
supporting
children’s
nature
play.

 4.

 Identifying
specific
recommended
children’s
reading
resources
to
support
positive
nature
 experiences
may
help
parents
and
care
givers
make
good
choices.

Perceived
Distance
to
Nature
 Of
greatest
interest
to
the
researchers
in
this
study
was
the
predictive
strength
of
the
perceived
 distance
to
nature.
In
developing
the
scale,
it
was
acknowledged
that
the
most
elusive
concept
was
the
 “nature
of
nature”
in
American
discourse.
It
is
known
that
there
are
a
variety
of
well‐researched
 theories
on
how
the
perception
of
nature
informs
behavior
and
belief.
For
the
purposes
of
this
study,
 however,
we
reduced
this
dimension
to
a
single
scale
that
sought
to
understand
what
people
believed
 the
“distance
to
appropriate
nature”
was.
We
speculated
at
the
time
that
the
degree
of
urbanity
could
 be
an
impact
and,
as
mentioned
earlier,
that
some
cities
may
be
considered
more
“nature‐like”
than
 developed
sub‐divisions.
We
were
surprised
to
see
the
predictive
strength
of
this
scale
(explaining
17%
 of
the
variance
in
parental
control
scales),
given
that
there
was
no
relationship
between
perceived
 distance
to
nature
and
the
size
of
community
in
which
someone
lived.
These
results
demonstrate
that
 the
perception
of
what
nature
is
appropriate
for
children
may
be
a
structural
limitation
for
some
people
 and
may
directly
impact
their
willingness
to
encourage
children’s
nature
play.
This
rough
scale
merely
 indicates
a
potentially
rich
source
of
new
research
into
how
Americans
perceive
their
separation
from
 nature
and
how
those
discourses
are
shaping
the
communities
where
we
raise
our
children.
 It
is
important
to
note
in
these
interpretations,
and
as
noted
in
the
findings,
that
we
cannot
confirm
 whether
anything
natural
is,
in
fact,
the
distance
claimed
by
our
participants.
But
given
the
lack
of
 correlation
with
urbanity,
this
distance
may
be
perceived
as,
or
may
in
fact
represent,
small
areas
 without
trees,
grass
or
other
natural
features.
These
may
exist
in
communities
of
all
scales,
from
small
 towns
to
metropolitan
areas.
Further
geographic
study
of
these
questions
is
certainly
warranted,
not
 only
to
identify
the
zones,
but
also
to
confirm
whether
those
living
in
those
areas
believe
they
are
 disconnected
from
nature
and
whether
their
beliefs
about
the
value
of
nature
experiences
for
children
 correspond
with
these
results.
 
Recommendations:
 1.
 Developing
targeted
media
communications
that
link
the
value
of
nature
experiences
for
 children
with
a
story
that
“nature
is
all
around
you”
may
help
those
who
feel
nature
is
near
 but
not
close
enough
to
feel
they
can
surmount
the
travel
barrier
for
their
children.
In
this
 case,
focusing
on
urban
nature
(e.g.,
the
resilience
of
plants,
the
ubiquity
of
birds)
may
help
 adults
recognize
beneficial
play
places
for
children
that
they
may
have
overlooked.

 2.
 Identifying
pilot
“disconnected
island
communities”
in
order
to
understand
their
cultural
 narratives
around
children’s
nature
play,
their
beliefs
and
the
perceived
barriers
they
may
 have
surrounding
encouraging
children’s
nature
play
are
all
warranted.
 3.

 Further
mixed‐methods
research
to
determine
the
discourses
that
inform
perceived
distance
 to
nature
is
warranted
to
guide
future
communications.

Parenthood,
Nature
and
the
Difference
between
Women
and
Men
 Perhaps
it
should
not
have
come
as
a
surprise
that
mothers
were
more
likely
than
fathers
to
support
 children’s
nature
experiences,
that
parents
in
general
are
more
supportive
than
other
adults,
and
that
 women
are
more
likely
to
believe
that
nature
is
valuable
to
children.
This
divide
in
beliefs
may
have
 some
basis
in
the
stereotypes
around
“traditional
family
values”
where
women
are
characterized
as
 nurturing
children’s
independent
play,
while
men
are
often
stereotyped
as
competitors
who
may
see
 unstructured
play
as
less
valuable
than
organized
activity.
This
speculation,
however,
does
little
to
 explain
why
sex
has
an
impact
on
beliefs.
These
data
do
suggest
that
the
children
and
nature
movement
 Institute for Learning Innovation

30

Results
of
the
2010
C&NN
EC‐NES
National
Survey


appears
to
be
highly
appealing
to
families
and
the
social
norms
associated
with
encouraging
children
to
 play
in
nature
all
bode
well
for
the
movement.

In
spite
of
adults’
varied
experiences
with
nature
in
the
 past,
most
parents
in
this
study
were
more
likely
to
prioritize
nature
and
feel
that
the
benefits
slightly
 outweigh
the
risks.

 We
also
speculate
that
the
majority
of
outdoor
adventurers
highlighted
in
the
press
tend
to
fall
into
the
 stereotypical
“male
loner”
like
Bear
Grylls,
while
iconic
women
in
the
wild
nature
media
field
tend
 toward
Jane
Goodall’s
family
nurturing
character.
These
stereotypes
may
have
some
traction
when
the
 participants
in
this
survey
were
considering
what
they
(as
a
man
or
woman)
consider
a
nature
 experience
and
where
they
perceived
risk.
We
cannot
determine
from
these
data
if
fathers
also
believe
 they
have
a
higher
degree
of
responsibility
as
protectors
of
their
children,
and
therefore
might
be
likely
 to
perceive
risks
to
be
more
threatening
than
mothers.
Further
qualitative
and
quantitative
evaluation
 of
the
survey
instrument
to
clarify
whether
there
are
sex
differences
in
how
the
statements
about
 nature
are
interpreted
in
terms
of
risk
may
help
to
refine
further
interpretation
of
these
data.
 Recommendations:
 1.
 Developing
programs
aimed
at
mothers
and
“Mr.
Mom’s”
may
have
a
higher
likelihood
of
 success
than
those
that
aim
at
the
traditional
father
or
adults
in
general.

 2.
 Developing
targeted
communications
toward
fathers
as
mentors
of
strong
healthy
children
 may
help
redress
the
perceived
risks.
 3.

 Reviewing
risk
perception,
masculinity
and
feminist
studies
on
issues
of
risk
perception,
 parenthood
and
sex‐based
stereotyping
may
provide
more
insights
into
these
data.

Educational
Achievement,
Income
and
Culture
 As
noted
in
the
findings,
educational
achievement
and
income
all
have
a
linear
relationship
to
 supporting
children’s
nature
experiences;
the
higher
education
or
income,
the
more
likely
someone
is
to
 support
all
aspects
of
the
EC‐NES
scale.
Culture,
on
the
other
hand,
as
operationalized
in
this
survey
as
 race
or
Hispanic
heritage,
appears
to
have
a
moderating
impact
on
beliefs
about
nature
experiences,
 both
positive
and
negative.
We
infer
from
these
data
that
cultural
stories
and
practices
are
as
likely
to
 impact
beliefs
as
any
public
value
and
acknowledge
that
America
blends
many
cultures
into
a
rough
 scale
that
cannot
truly
represent
the
diversity
of
its
people.
These
data
merely
suggest
that
those
in
the
 low
income/low
education
communities
are
also
likely
more
at
risk
for
not
helping
their
children
achieve
 their
potential
because
they
may
not
place
emphasis
on
what
children
gain
from
having
time
in
nature.
 We
do
not
suggest
that
race
or
ethnicity
alone
should
be
used
to
assess
a
specific
community.
These
 data
suggest
that
local
cultural
factors
may
be
at
play,
and
that
some
values
surrounding
nature
 experiences
are
deep‐seated
within
a
cultural
community.

We
believe
that
anthropological
study
of
 community
cultures
may
be
more
useful
for
understanding
how
nature
experiences
are
characterized
in
 low‐income/low
academic
achievement
communities
to
identify
the
specific
barriers
that
prevent
 children
from
having
opportunities
with
nature.
 Recommendations:
 1.
 Low
income
and
low
academic
achievement
communities
should
be
considered
a
priority
for
 the
children
and
nature
movement.
Investment
in
programs
aimed
at
understanding
cultural
 barriers
and
aid
in
mentoring/modeling
behaviors
for
children
and
their
caregivers
in
these
 communities
are
likely
to
help
increase
the
opportunities
for
children
in
these
communities.
 2.
 Identifying
positive
cultural
stories
about
the
value
of
nature
for
children
may
help
shift
 normative
beliefs
in
society.
For
example,
traditional
stories
about
the
positive
value
of
 nature
from
Africa
and
Latin
America
may
aid
in
establishing
models
and
recovering
stories
 that
may
have
been
suppressed
as
these
communities
became
part
of
American
society.

American
Attitudes
Toward
Children’s
Nature
Experiences

31
































































June
2010


3.

 Replicating
this
study
with
substantially
larger
sample
sizes
using
a
mixed‐methods
approach
 with
specific
targeted
sub‐populations
that
are
culturally
coherent
and
at
the
same
 economic
and
educational
strata
in
American
society
may
illuminate
more
nuances
about
 the
barriers
to
believing
that
nature
experiences
can
help
children
develop
more
fully.

Age
 The
responses
based
on
age
indicate
that
values
and
beliefs
concerning
children
and
nature
are
more
 strongly
held
by
those
in
older
generations
(Table
14).
It
is
important
to
note
that
beliefs
and
values
that
 are
commonly
held
among
a
generation
can
be
an
artifact
of
aging
but
can
also
be
a
function
of
the
 social
conditions
surrounding
that
generation’s
specific
experiences
as
they
came
of
age.
These
results
 can
account
for
greater
support
of
the
beliefs
and
values
of
the
children
and
nature
movement
by
age,
 but
cannot
be
inferred
to
be
solely
a
function
of
aging.
Demographic
changes
associated
with
urban
 expansion,
increased
population
in
cities,
the
social
narratives
surrounding
the
return
to
the
earth
 movement
that
shaped
the
seventies
consciousness,
the
republican
retrenchment
of
the
‘80s,
the
 polarization
of
political
values
in
the
‘90s,
and
the
entry
into
the
war
years
of
the
first
decade
of
the
21st
 century
are
all
social
changes
that
have
had
an
incredible
impact
on
how
beliefs
and
values
are
 supported.
These
data
suggest
that
the
children
and
nature
movement
is
ideally
placed
for
support
from
 the
older
demographic,
but
also
demonstrates
reason
for
concern
about
the
future
of
the
movement.
 Recommendations:
 1.
 To
assess
the
possible
risk
or
durability
of
the
movement,
further
mixed‐methods
research
 into
how
values
about
children’s
nature
experiences
were
developed
by
each
generation,
 how
their
developmental
experiences
impacted
those
values,
and
how
these
values
are
 attached
to
other
concerns
will
help
with
interpretation
of
these
results.

 2.
 Developing
programs
that
target
older
adults
in
mentoring
youth
experiences
should
be
well
 received
and
can
be
easily
implemented
and
will
find
a
supportive
self‐motivated
audience.
 3.

 Focused
programs
for
Gen
X
and
Gen
Y
women
during
their
parenting
years
may
help
reduce
 resistance
because
women
with
children
in
their
households
are
more
likely
to
support
 beliefs
that
nature
experiences
are
valuable
for
their
children.

Public
Policy
 These
data
demonstrate
the
views
of
the
American
population.
We
cannot
infer
that
these
values
are
 consistent
for
the
Americans
specifically
charged
with
shaping
the
cities
where
children
are
raised,
nor
 those
who
promote
learning,
health
and
well‐being
strategies
in
those
communities.
Determining
the
 values
and
beliefs
within
organizations,
such
as
urban
planners,
architects,
developers,
the
health
care
 professions,
the
education
community
and
media
professionals
will
help
to
identify
possible
action
steps
 that
may
help
redress
concerns
about
perceived
separation
from
nature,
and
how
the
nature
that
is
 around
people
can
be
used
by
children.

 Limitations
of
this
study
 The
data
collected
for
this
study
represents
a
first
baseline
assessment
of
adult
perceptions
regarding
 their
willingness
to
support
children’s
outdoor
activity.

It
does
not
address
other
non‐nature
related
 experiences
that
may
also
support
childhood
development
or
the
potential
trade‐offs
that
adults
might
 make
when
choosing
to
support
any
type
of
experience.

Further
comparative
research
based
on
these
 instruments
may
help
to
determine
whether
other
developmental
experiences
are
similarly
supported
 or
ranked
at
a
higher
or
lower
importance
than
nature
contact.


While
it
might
be
surmised
that
these
 results
were
more
positive
than
anticipated,
the
variation
in
responses
based
on
demographic,
regional,
 and
economic
differences
suggest
that
Americans
value
nature
experiences
differently.

Institute for Learning Innovation

32

Results
of
the
2010
C&NN
EC‐NES
National
Survey


We
caution
that
average
values
represented
in
these
results
are
internally
relative
to
what
these
scales
 measure.
As
noted
and
where
possible,
affirmatively
written
statements
were
used
when
there
was
no
 variation
with
a
negatively
worded
statement
assessing
the
same
construct.
It
is
possible
that
supporting
 nature
contact
in
a
survey
is
a
socially
acceptable
response
that
does
not
represent
actual
behavior.

 This
study
assessed
attitudes
and
confirmed
that
nature
contact
is
perceived
as
valuable
for
children,
 not
any
specific
behavior.

We
do
not
conclude
that
nature
experiences
are
more
or
less
valuable
than
 any
other
experience
and
caution
against
any
inference
that
we
imply
such
a
conclusion.

 We
also
note
that
the
results
may
indicate
a
generally
positive
perception
toward
supporting
nature
 experiences,
but
draw
attention
to
the
comparison
between
what
people
are
allowing
their
children
to
 do
and
their
perceptions
about
supporting
children’s
experience.


The
instrument
developed
for
this
 study
can
support
future
exploration
with
small
groups
and
national
benchmarking
studies,
with
a
 specific
focus
on
how
narratives
associated
with
dimensions
such
as
priority
and
safety
may
change
over
 time,
and
what
impact
such
narratives
have
on
actual
encouragement.


Although
the
results
may
 appear,
at
first
pass,
to
be
generally
positive,
most
responses
still
remain
only
marginally
beyond
neutral
 and
certainly
have
the
capacity
to
change
within
any
one
of
the
dimensions
reported
here.

Conclusion
 Americans
have
the
best
of
intentions
to
support
children’s
nature
contact,
but
face
a
variety
of
belief
 and
control
barriers
that
limit
their
ability
to
support
these
intentions.
One
of
the
greatest
barriers
 emerging
from
these
results
is
the
perception
that
nature
is
far
away
and
difficult
to
access,
followed
 closely
by
a
concern
for
children’s
safety
in
nature
places,
and
the
general
lack
in
belief
that
telling
 stories
about
nature
experiences
can
help
encourage
children
to
see
nature
as
an
important
part
of
their
 lives.
These
deficits
may
pose
a
limit
on
whether
they
are
willing
to
act
on
these
beliefs,
but
all
of
these
 external
factors
can
be
easily
overcome
through
efforts
of
a
targeted
national
campaign.
 Americans
who
have
not
achieved
high
levels
of
educational
achievement,
low‐income
families,
and,
to
 some
extent,
minority
communities
are
least
likely
to
support
the
value
of
nature
experiences
for
 children.
Focusing
efforts
on
these
historically
disadvantaged
communities
can
go
a
long
way
toward
 creating
opportunities
for
children
that
do
not
require
additional
assets
or
means
to
experience
nature.
 In
particular,
these
diverse
communities
may
benefit
from
learning
how
to
act
as
mentors
through
 personal
storytelling
with
a
resulting
increase
in
the
likelihood
that
children
will
desire
to
have
nature
 experiences,
and
will
gain
an
educational
and
physical
advantage
that
may
help
them
in
the
long
term.
 Of
greatest
concern
in
these
results
were
the
findings
that
adults
are
extremely
concerned
about
the
 risks
to
children
associated
with
free‐flowing
streams,
woods,
mountains
and
wild
nature.
A
two‐ pronged
approach
around
talking
about
the
value
of
more
proximate
nature
experiences
is
advised.
The
 first
prong
might
address
the
value
of
free‐play
in
green
spaces
and
planned
nature
places
to
increase
 support
for
nature
contact
without
the
use
of
“high‐risk”
words.
A
secondary
effort
focusing
on
the
 safety
of
accompanied
experiences
with
mentors
or
other
significant
adults
to
more
wild
nature
areas
 may
be
more
acceptable
to
the
majority
of
Americans
and
may
help
to
increase
acceptance
of
the
 research
that
suggests
the
importance
of
nature
in
healthy
development
of
children.
 Lastly,
two
communities
appear
to
be
natural
supporters
for
the
movement:
American
Indian/First
 Nations
peoples
and
older
Americans
more
strongly
support
the
value
of
nature
experiences
for
 children’s
well‐being.
These
two
communities
have
substantial
legacy
concerns
for
the
shaping
of
 America
and
offer
an
opportunity
for
links
to
an
American
land
ethic
that
can
increase
the
appeal
of
 children’s
nature
experiences
for
all
Americans.

American
Attitudes
Toward
Children’s
Nature
Experiences

33
































































June
2010


Appendix
A:
 Encouraging
Children’s
Nature
Experiences
Scale
[EC‐NES]
 Fraser,
Heimlich
&Yocco
(2010)


©
Institute
for
Learning
Innovation
(2010)
 
 
 
 1)

 Please
select
the
category
that
includes
the
year
you
were
born:
 __1993
–
2009
 __1977
–
1992

 __1965
–
1976

 __1955
–
1964

 __1946
–
1954

 __1937
–
1945

 __Born
1936
or
earlier
 
 2)

 In
which
state/province
is
your
home
______?
 
 
 3)

 Which
television
network
do
you
rely
on
as
your
primary
source
for
TV
news?
(Select
one)
 __ABC
 __CBS
 __CW
 __FOX
 __NBC
 __PBS
 __Cable
news
stations
(e.g.
CNN,
MSNBC,
etc.)
 __Other,
please
specify
_____________________
 
 
 4)

 Have
you
watched
at
least
one
television
show
about
nature
from
beginning
to
end
in
the
past
 two
years?
 __Yes
 __No
 
 
 5)

 Have
you
visited
a
national,
state,
or
regional
nature
park
in
the
past
two
years?
 __Yes
 __No

Institute for Learning Innovation

34

Results
of
the
2010
C&NN
EC‐NES
National
Survey


6)

 For
the
following
statements,
please
rate
how
strongly
you
agree
or
disagree
with
each
one.


 If
you
strongly
disagree
with
the
statement,
you’d
circle
a
1.

If
you
strongly
agree
with
the
 statement,
you’d
circle
a
7.

If
you
are
somewhere
in
between
you
would
circle
a
3,
4,
or
5.

Strongly
 agree

Strongly
 disagree

All
children
learn
from
nature
whenever
they
are
outdoors
 Being
in
nature
helps
children
learn
how
things
work
 Being
in
a
nature
setting
helps
a
child
develop
emotionally
 Free
play
outdoors
helps
children
learn
self
control
 Children
develop
good
memory
skills
by
being
in
nature
 Children
develop
their
thinking
ability
by
being
in
nature
 Playing
outdoors
helps
children
learn
to
solve
problems
 Children
learn
how
to
learn
by
themselves
when
they
play
in
nature

1
 1
 1
 1
 1
 1
 1
 1

2
 2
 2
 2
 2
 2
 2
 2

3
 3
 3
 3
 3
 3
 3
 3

4
 4
 4
 4
 4
 4
 4
 4

5
 5
 5
 5
 5
 5
 5
 5

6
 6
 6
 6
 6
 6
 6
 6

7
 7
 7
 7
 7
 7
 7
 7

7)
 For
the
following
statements,
please
rate
how
strongly
you
agree
or
disagree
with
each
one.
 If
you
strongly
disagree
with
the
statement,
you’d
circle
a
1.

If
you
strongly
agree
with
the
 statement,
you’d
circle
a
7.

If
you
are
somewhere
in
between
you
would
circle
a
3,
4,
or
5.
 Strongly
 disagree

Strongly
 agree

The
costs
of
a
child
being
hurt
outdoors
exceed
the
benefits
 It
is
difficult
to
get
children
to
play
outdoors
 If
day
care
providers
don’t
take
kids
outside,
why
should
I
 Schools
don’t
care
about
kids
being
outside,
so
why
should
I
 Children
don’t
get
a
lot
of
benefit
from
being
in
nature
 Being
outside
can
expose
a
child
to
bad
germs
and
disease

1
 1
 1
 1
 1
 1

2
 2
 2
 2
 2
 2

3
 3
 3
 3
 3
 3

4
 4
 4
 4
 4
 4

5
 5
 5
 5
 5
 5

6
 6
 6
 6
 6
 6

7
 7
 7
 7
 7
 7

8)
 For
the
following
statements,
please
rate
how
strongly
you
agree
or
disagree
with
each
one.


 If
you
strongly
disagree
with
the
statement,
you’d
circle
a
1.

If
you
strongly
agree
with
the
 statement,
you’d
circle
a
7.

If
you
are
somewhere
in
between
you
would
circle
a
3,
4,
or
5.
 Strongly
 disagree

Strongly
 agree

I
don’t
feel
comfortable
in
nature
 Playgrounds
are
safer
for
children
than
natural
areas
 It
is
a
challenge
to
find
a
safe
place
to
take
children
to
play
in
nature
 It
is
a
challenge
to
find
a
safe
place
to
take
children
to
play
outdoors
 Children
need
to
be
supervised
at
all
times
when
they
play
outdoors
 The
costs
of
a
child
being
hurt
outdoors
exceed
the
benefits
 I
am
concerned
about
a
child
getting
hurt
when
they
play
outdoors
 I
am
concerned
about
a
child
getting
hurt
when
they
play
in
nature
 Bad
people
can
take
advantage
of
children
when
they
play
in
nature

1
 1
 1
 1
 1
 1
 1
 1
 1

2
 2
 2
 2
 2
 2
 2
 2
 2

3
 3
 3
 3
 3
 3
 3
 3
 3

4
 4
 4
 4
 4
 4
 4
 4
 4

5
 5
 5
 5
 5
 5
 5
 5
 5

6
 6
 6
 6
 6
 6
 6
 6
 6

7
 7
 7
 7
 7
 7
 7
 7
 7

American
Attitudes
Toward
Children’s
Nature
Experiences

35
































































June
2010


9)
 For
the
following
statements,
please
rate
how
strongly
you
agree
or
disagree
with
each
one.


 If
you
strongly
disagree
with
the
statement,
you’d
circle
a
1.

If
you
strongly
agree
with
the
 statement,
you’d
circle
a
7.

If
you
are
somewhere
in
between
you
would
circle
a
3,
4,
or
5.

Strongly
 disagree

Children
don’t
really
need
to
be
outdoors
 Children
don’t
really
need
to
be
in
nature
 There
is
nothing
to
learn
from
playing
outside
that
can’t
be
taught
in
school
 The
effort
to
have
children
in
nature
is
not
worth
the
benefits

1
 1
 1
 1

Strongly
 agree

2
 2
 2
 2

3
 3
 3
 3

4
 4
 4
 4

5
 5
 5
 5

6
 6
 6
 6

7
 7
 7
 7

10)
For
the
following
statements,
please
rate
how
strongly
you
agree
or
disagree
with
each
one.


 If
you
strongly
disagree
with
the
statement,
you’d
circle
a
1.

If
you
strongly
agree
with
the
 statement,
you’d
circle
a
7.

If
you
are
somewhere
in
between
you
would
circle
a
3,
4,
or
5.
 Strongly
 disagree

Strongly
 agree

Free‐play
in
a
natural
area
helps
a
child
become
more
creative
 Child’s
play
in
a
natural
area
is
important
in
helping
a
child
develop
 Children
are
healthier
when
they
play
in
nature
 Playing
in
nature
is
important
for
a
child’s
physical
health
 Free‐play
in
natural
settings
encourages
vigorous
activity
for
children
 Free‐play
in
nature
is
important
for
a
child’s
physical
well‐being
 Vigorous
activity
in
natural
settings
is
good
for
children
 Physical
fitness
is
an
important
benefit
of
children
playing
in
nature
 Playing
in
nature
has
a
positive
impact
on
a
child’s
mental
health
 Seeing
trees
and
plants
has
a
positive
impact
on
a
child’s
mental
health

1
 1
 1
 1
 1
 1
 1
 1
 1
 1

2
 2
 2
 2
 2
 2
 2
 2
 2
 2

3
 3
 3
 3
 3
 3
 3
 3
 3
 3

4
 4
 4
 4
 4
 4
 4
 4
 4
 4

5
 5
 5
 5
 5
 5
 5
 5
 5
 5

6
 6
 6
 6
 6
 6
 6
 6
 6
 6

7
 7
 7
 7
 7
 7
 7
 7
 7
 7

11)
For
the
following
statements,
please
rate
how
strongly
you
agree
or
disagree
with
each
one.


 If
you
strongly
disagree
with
the
statement,
you’d
circle
a
1.

If
you
strongly
agree
with
the
 statement,
you’d
circle
a
7.

If
you
are
somewhere
in
between
you
would
circle
a
3,
4,
or
5.
 
 Strongly
 disagree

Strongly
 agree

Children
build
confidence
in
themselves
when
they
are
allowed
to
play
in
nature
 Children
build
their
self‐esteem
when
they
are
allowed
to
play
by
themselves
in
 nature
 Free‐play
in
nature
helps
a
child
become
more
independent
 Free‐play
in
natural
areas
gives
a
child
a
greater
sense
of
what
they
can
control
 Children
improve
their
ability
to
concentrate
when
they
can
play
in
nature
 Children
would
be
less
obese
if
they
played
in
nature
more
often
 Playing
in
a
natural
area
contributes
to
a
sense
of
belonging
 Playing
in
natural
areas
helps
connect
children
to
their
community
 Playing
in
natural
areas
helps
children
build
an
awareness
of
their
own
abilities

1
 1

2
 2

3
 3

4
 4

5
 5

6
 6

7
 7

1
 1
 1
 1
 1
 1
 1

2
 2
 2
 2
 2
 2
 2

3
 3
 3
 3
 3
 3
 3

4
 4
 4
 4
 4
 4
 4

5
 5
 5
 5
 5
 5
 5

6
 6
 6
 6
 6
 6
 6

7
 7
 7
 7
 7
 7
 7

Institute for Learning Innovation

36

Results
of
the
2010
C&NN
EC‐NES
National
Survey


12)
For
the
following
statements,
please
rate
how
strongly
you
agree
or
disagree
with
each
one.


 If
you
strongly
disagree
with
the
statement,
you’d
circle
a
1.

If
you
strongly
agree
with
the
 statement,
you’d
circle
a
7.

If
you
are
somewhere
in
between
you
would
circle
a
3,
4,
or
5.
 
 Strongly
 disagree

Strongly
 agree

Playing
in
natural
areas
helps
children
develop
better
coordination
 The
skills
a
child
gains
from
playing
in
a
natural
area
are
unique
 Playing
in
nature
helps
children
develop
physical
strength
 Free‐play
in
natural
settings
is
important
for
children
learning
to
play
with
others
 Children
learn
about
how
society
works
when
they
play
with
other
children
in
 natural
areas
 Cooperation
is
an
important
ability
learned
by
children
when
they
play
together
in
 nature

1
 1
 1
 1
 1

2
 2
 2
 2
 2

3
 3
 3
 3
 3

4
 4
 4
 4
 4

5
 5
 5
 5
 5

6
 6
 6
 6
 6

7
 7
 7
 7
 7

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

13)
For
the
following
statements,
please
rate
how
strongly
you
agree
or
disagree
with
each
one.


 If
you
strongly
disagree
with
the
statement,
you’d
circle
a
1.

If
you
strongly
agree
with
the
 statement,
you’d
circle
a
7.

If
you
are
somewhere
in
between
you
would
circle
a
3,
4,
or
5.
 
 Strongly
 disagree

Strongly
 agree

I
will
make
sure
the
children
in
my
life
have
opportunities
to
play
outdoors
 I
will
take
children
to
nature
places
where
they
can
play
 I
will
try
to
help
children
learn
to
be
good
members
of
society
 I
will
make
sure
the
children
in
my
life
respect
private
property
 I
will
make
sure
the
children
in
my
life
learn
to
take
care
of
nature
 I
will
advocate
for
protecting
the
natural
areas
in
our
community

1
 1
 1
 1
 1
 1

2
 2
 2
 2
 2
 2

3
 3
 3
 3
 3
 3

4
 4
 4
 4
 4
 4

5
 5
 5
 5
 5
 5

6
 6
 6
 6
 6
 6

7
 7
 7
 7
 7
 7

14)
For
the
following
statements,
please
rate
how
strongly
you
agree
or
disagree
with
each
one.


 If
you
strongly
disagree
with
the
statement,
you’d
circle
a
1.

If
you
strongly
agree
with
the
 statement,
you’d
circle
a
7.

If
you
are
somewhere
in
between
you
would
circle
a
3,
4,
or
5.
 
 Strongly
 disagree

Strongly
 agree

I
tell
stories
about
my
personal
experiences
with
nature
 I
tell
stories
about
nature
to
build
family
bonds
 I
share
stories
about
nature
with
children
 I
think
it
is
important
for
children
to
hear
stories
about
nature
 I
read
fictional
stories
about
nature
to
children
 I
read
fictional
stories
about
animals
to
children

1
 1
 1
 1
 1
 1

2
 2
 2
 2
 2
 2

3
 3
 3
 3
 3
 3

4
 4
 4
 4
 4
 4

5
 5
 5
 5
 5
 5

6
 6
 6
 6
 6
 6

7
 7
 7
 7
 7
 7

American
Attitudes
Toward
Children’s
Nature
Experiences

37
































































June
2010


15)
For
the
following
statements,
please
rate
how
strongly
you
agree
or
disagree
with
each
one.


 If
you
strongly
disagree
with
the
statement,
you’d
circle
a
1.

If
you
strongly
agree
with
the
 statement,
you’d
circle
a
7.

If
you
are
somewhere
in
between
you
would
circle
a
3,
4,
or
5.
 
 Strongly

Strongly

disagree
 agree
 
 By
being
outdoors,
children
learn
to
appreciate
what
nature
provides
 1
 2
 3
 4
 5
 6
 7
 Nature
helps
children
to
learn
about
their
role
in
the
“circle
of
life”
 1
 2
 3
 4
 5
 6
 7
 By
being
outdoors,
children
learn
about
how
nature
works
 1
 2
 3
 4
 5
 6
 7
 Nature
experiences
help
children
learn
to
care
about
wild
animals
 1
 2
 3
 4
 5
 6
 7
 Being
in
nature
helps
children
develop
their
own
values
 1
 2
 3
 4
 5
 6
 7
 Children
learn
about
their
world
better
by
being
outdoors
 1
 2
 3
 4
 5
 6
 7
 Children
learn
to
care
for
nature
when
they
play
outdoors
 1
 2
 3
 4
 5
 6
 7
 
 
 16)
How
far
from
your
home
is
the
closest
nature
place
that
you
think
is
appropriate
for
children’s
 play?
(Select
One)
 __Just
outside
our
door
 __5
minute
walk
 __15
minute
walk
 __15
minutes
by
car
 __30
minutes
by
car
 __30
minutes
by
bus
(don’t
have
a
car)
 __1
hour
(any
type
of
transportation)
 __More
than
90
minutes
to
get
to
nature
from
where
I
live
 
 17)
Please
provide
a
brief
description
of
the
nature
place
that
best
fits
the
place
you
described
in
the
 previous
question:

 
 
 
 18)

When
you
were
a
child,
which
of
these
places
were
you
allowed
to
play
unsupervised?

(Choose
all
 that
apply)
 
 Yes
 No
 At
home
or
my
friends
home
indoors
 ___
 ___
 Indoor
activity
area
 ___
 ___
 School
playground
 ___
 ___
 Indoor
after
school
club
 ___
 ___
 Indoor
sports
center
 ___
 ___
 In
the
streets
near
my
home
 ___
 ___
 Garden
 ___
 ___
 School
playing
fields
 ___
 ___
 Outdoor
adventure
playground
 ___
 ___
 Woods
 ___
 ___
 Shrubland/fields/farmland
 ___
 ___
 Riverside/creekside/pond
 ___
 ___
 Mountains/grassy
hills/other
wild
spaces
 ___
 ___
 Institute for Learning Innovation

38

Results
of
the
2010
C&NN
EC‐NES
National
Survey


19)
In
which
of
these
places
would
you
allow
your
child
aged
7‐11
to
play
unsupervised?


 (Choose
all
that
apply)
 
 Yes
 No
 At
home
or
my
friend’s
home
indoors
 ___
 ___
 Indoor
activity
area
 ___
 ___
 School
playground
 ___
 ___
 Indoor
after
school
club
 ___
 ___
 Indoor
sports
center
 ___
 ___
 In
the
streets
near
my
home
 ___
 ___
 Garden
 ___
 ___
 School
playing
fields
 ___
 ___
 Outdoor
adventure
playground
 ___
 ___
 Woods
 ___
 ___
 Shrubland/fields/farmland
 ___
 ___
 Riverside/creekside/pond
 ___
 ___
 Mountains/grassy
hills/other
wild
spaces
 ___
 ___

American
Attitudes
Toward
Children’s
Nature
Experiences

39
































































June
2010


Please
tell
us
some
more
about
yourself:
 
 20)
Are
you
Spanish/Hispanic/Latino?
 ___No,
not
Spanish/Hispanic/Latino
 ___Yes,
Mexican,
Mexican
American,
Chicano
 ___Yes,
Puerto
Rican
 ___Yes,
Cuban
 ___Yes,
Other
Spanish/Hispanic/Latino
 ___Prefer
not
to
answer
 
 
21)
Which
of
the
following
best
describes
your
family
heritage?

(select
all
that
apply)
 ___White/Caucasian
 ___Black/African
American
 ___American
Indian/First
Nations
 ___Asian/Pacific
Islander
 ___Other
 ___Prefer
not
to
answer
 
 22)
What
is
your
highest
level
of
academic
achievement?
 ___Some
high
school

 ___High
school
diploma/GED
 ___Some
college
 ___Undergraduate
degree
 ___Some
post‐graduate
studies
 ___Post‐graduate
degree
 
 23)
Are
you
now,
or
have
you
been
a
parent/guardian
of
children?
 Yes
 
 
 No
 
 
 24)
Do
you
have
a
child
under
17
who
lives
in
your
household?
 Yes
 
 
 No
 
 
 25)
Which
of
the
following
best
represents
your
household
income
last
year
before
taxes?
 ___Less
than
$25,000
 ___$25,000‐$34,999
 ___$35,000‐$49,999
 ___$50,000‐$74,999
 ___$75,000‐$99,999
 ___$100,000‐149,000
 ___$150,000‐199,000
 ___$200,000
or
more
 
 26)
Are
you?

 ___Male
 ___Female

 ___Prefer
not
to
answer
 
 
 Institute for Learning Innovation

40

Results
of
the
2010
C&NN
EC‐NES
National
Survey


Appendix
B:
 Descriptive
Statistics

Table
B.1:
Frequency
and
percent
of
participants
by
age
group,
sex,
and
parental
status

Total
 (N=2138)

Age
Group
 18‐33
 Years

34‐45
 Years

46‐55
 Years

Sex

56‐64
 Years

65‐73
 Years

74+
 Years

Parent/guardian

Child
at
 home
 Yes

Male

Female

Yes

Frequency

470

604

432

253

180

199

981

1148

1479

745

Percent

22.0

28.3

20.2

11.8

8.4

9.3

45.9

53.7

69.2

34.8

Note:
For
sex,
percent
does
not
equal
100
due
to
0.4%
of
participantspreferring
not
to
answer.

The
estimated
intersex
and
transgender
 population
in
America
may
be
as
high
as
1:500
and
would
be
consistent
with
these
responses.

Table
B.2:
Frequency
and
percent
of
participants
by
residence/community
size
 How
would
you
describe
your
current
residence
or
community

Suburb

(N=2138)

City
 250,000+

City
 100,000‐ 249,000

City
 50,000‐ 99,999

Small
city
 25,000‐ 49,999

Town
 10,000‐ 24,999

Town
 5,000‐ 9,999

Town
 under
 5,000

Farm/ rural
 area

Yes

Frequency

457

310

258

309

259

153

178

214

1145

Percent

21.4

14.5

12.1

14.5

12.1

7.2

8.3

10.0

53.6

Total

Table
B.3:
Percent
and
frequency
of
participants
by
income
 Total
 (N=2138)

Income
 Under
 $25,000

$25,000‐ $34,999

$35,000‐ $49,999

$50,000‐ $74,999

$75,000‐ $99,999

$100,000‐ $149,999

$150,000‐ $200,000

$200,000
+

Frequency

332

242

327

447

279

190

44

34

Percent

15.5

11.3

15.3

20.9

13.0

8.9

2.1

1.6

Note:
percent
does
not
equal
100
because
11.4%
of
respondentschose
“Prefer
not
to
answer.”

These
responses
are
consistent
with
other
 consumer
surveys,
but
the
distribution
of
all
respondents
did
represent
a
normal
distribution
and
consistent
with
recent
census
data.

Table
B.4:
Percent
and
frequency
of
participants
by
education,
park
visitorship,
and
watching
a
nature
 show
in
the
past
two
years
 Education
 Total

H.S.
 diploma/

Leisure
Learning

College
 degree

Some
post‐ graduate
 studies

Post‐ graduate
 degree

Visited
 parks

Watch
 nature
 shows

(N=2138)

Some
 H.S.

GED

Some
 college

Frequency

66

414

708

528

141

281

1193

1570

Percent

3.1

19.4

33.1

24.7

6.6

13.1

55.8

73.4

American
Attitudes
Toward
Children’s
Nature
Experiences

41
































































June
2010


Table
B.5:
Summated
scale
 
by
age
group

18‐33
Years

34‐45
Years

46‐55
Years

56‐64
Years

65‐73
Years

74+
Years

470

604

432

253

180

199

22.0

28.3

20.2

11.8

8.4

9.3

Healthiness

5.59

6.33

6.46

6.50

6.61

6.41

Cognitive/Emotional
Growth

5.04

5.25

5.30

5.38

5.55

5.31

Emotional
Well‐being

5.35

5.55

5.57

5.69

5.81

5.66

Enhanced
Skills

5.44

5.66

5.64

5.73

5.91

5.79

Appreciation
of
Nature

5.64

5.80

5.92

5.96

6.13

5.98

Priority*

5.86

6.33

6.46

6.50

6.61

6.41

Child
Safety*

4.62

4.85

5.05

5.08

5.10

4.99

Storytelling

4.96

5.01

5.13

5.08

5.22

5.20

Effort/Risk*

5.54

5.83

5.98

6.05

6.11

5.90

Intentionality

5.81

6.11

6.18

6.23

6.38

6.29

Scale

%

N

Note:


 (1)
Values
based
on
responses
to
a
7
point
Likert‐type
scale
with
1
representing
strongly
disagree
and
7
representing
strongly
agree.

 (2)*
Designates
scale
contains
items
that
were
reverse
coded

 (3)

With
the
exception
of
the
storytelling
scale,
all
scale
 ’s
were
significantly
different
between
age
groups
at
p=.000

Table
B.6:
Summated
scale
 
choice
of
television
network
as
a
source
for
news

ABC,
CBS,
NBC

FOX

Cable
News

Other
(incl
PBS
&
CW)

N

1125

452

355

206

Scale











































%

52.6

21.1

16.6

9.6

Healthiness

5.89

5.86

5.86

5.71

Cognitive/Emotional
Growth

5.34

5.17

5.24

5.06

Emotional
Well‐being

5.63

5.50

5.52

5.38

Enhanced
Skills

5.72

5.60

5.59

5.44

Appreciation
of
Nature

5.92

5.80

5.81

5.66

Priority*

6.30

6.27

6.37

6.26

Child
Safety*

4.91

4.89

4.86

4.93

Storytelling

5.14

5.08

4.95

4.83

Effort/Risk*

5.86

5.85

5.91

5.74

Intentionality

6.13

6.08

6.13

6.04

Notes:


 (1)
Values
based
on
responses
to
a
7
point
Likert‐type
scale
with
1
representing
strongly
disagree
and
7
representing
strongly
agree.

 (2)*
Designates
scale
contains
items
that
were
reverse
coded

Institute for Learning Innovation

42

Results
of
the
2010
C&NN
EC‐NES
National
Survey


Table
B.7:
Summated
scale
 
by
income

$75,000 $100,000 $150,000‐ ‐ ‐ $200,000
 $99,999
 $149,999

$200,00 0
or
 more

Under
 $25,000

$25,000‐ $34,999

$35,000‐ $49,999

$50,000‐ $74,999

N

332

242

327

447

279

Scale


















%

15.5

11.3

15.3

20.9

13.0

8.9

2.1

1.6

Healthiness

5.90

5.79

5.81

5.85

5.90

6.04

6.04

5.61

Cognitive/Emotional
Growth

5.32

5.27

5.20

5.21

5.28

5.43

5.66

5.16

Emotional
Well‐
being

5.63

5.50

5.53

5.54

5.60

5.72

5.92

5.33

Enhanced
Skills

5,76

5.59

5.65

5.59

5.71

5.79

5.94

5.26

Appreciation
of
Nature

5.91

5.83

5.88

5.82

5.83

6.00

6.05

5.56

Priority*

6.15

6.21

6.25

6.39

6.37

6.47

6.45

6.03

Child
Safety*

4.76

4.71

4.88

4.97

4.98

5.03

5.31

4.74

Storytelling

5.00

5.11

5.12

5.02

5.12

5.26

5.17

5.20

Effort/Risk*

5.68

5.79

5.83

5.94

5.92

5.95

6.13

5.67

Intentionality

6.05

6.01

6.16

6.09

6.17

6.29

6.22

6.07

190

44

34

Notes:


 (1)
Values
based
on
responses
to
a
7
pointLikert‐type
scale
with
1
representing
strongly
disagree
and
7
representing
strongly
agree.

 (2)*
Designates
scale
contains
items
that
were
reverse
coded

Table
B.8:
Summated
scale
 
by
education

 
 Some
H.S.

H.S.
 diploma/
 GED

Some
 college

Undergraduate
 degree

Some
post‐ graduate
 studies

Post‐ graduate
 degree

N

66

414

708

528

141

281

Scale




















%

3.1

19.4

33.1

24.7

6.6

13.1

Healthiness

5.55

5.74

5.91

5.85

5.92

5.98

Cognitive/Emotional
Growth

4.81

5.21

5.26

5.22

5.34

5.49

Emotional
Well‐being

5.21

5.49

5.59

5.50

5.67

5.70

Enhanced
Skills

5.45

5.58

5.68

5.59

5.77

5.76

Appreciation
of
Nature

5.58

5.79

5.91

5.76

5.88

6.01

Priority*

5.83

6.13

6.37

6.30

6.45

6.43

Child
Safety*

4.63

4.68

4.95

4.90

5.09

5.06

Storytelling

4.70

5.10

5.06

4.96

5.31

5.21

Effort/Risk*

5.48

5.69

5.88

5.91

6.01

5.93

Intentionality

5.64

6.01

6.19

6.02

6.26

6.30

American
Attitudes
Toward
Children’s
Nature
Experiences

43
































































June
2010


Table
B.9:
What
were
you
allowed
to
do
and
what
would
you
allow
a
child
to
do
by
describe
your
 residence

Large
city
 250,000
+

City
 100,000‐ 249,000

City
 50,000‐ 99,999

Small
 city
 25,000
‐
 49,999

Town
 10,000‐ 24,999

Town
 5,000‐ 9,999

Town
 under
 5,000

Farm rural area

N

457

310

258

309

259

153

178

214

Location

%

21.4

14.5

12.1

14.5

12.1

7.2

8.3

10.0

At
home
or
 friends
home

Allowed
as
child

91.0

91.0

88.0

90.9

87.6

92.2

88.8

88.3

Would
allow
child

83.2

85.2

82.9

83.5

84.6

81.0

86.0

87.9

Indoor
activity
 area

Allowed
as
child

79.6

78.1

72.5

76.1

71.8

72.5

72.5

70.1

Would
allow
child

77.2

79.4

74.8

73.1

74.1

71.2

75.3

76.6

School
 playground

Allowed
as
child

83.6

87.7

82.2

80.6

81.1

79.7

78.1

78.5

Would
allow
child

73.5

73.2

67.1

66.3

64.1

60.1

69.7

65.4

Indoor
after
 school
club

Allowed
as
child

62.6

59.7

53.1

54.7

61.4

52.3

45.5

40.7

Would
allow
child

74.4

77.7

66.7

73.8

74.5

68.6

66.9

66.8

Indoor
sports
 center

Allowed
as
child

56.9

55.8

42.6

45.0

47.1

46.4

42.7

41.1

Would
allow
child

66.7

66.5

58.5

63.4

59.5

58.8

59.6

56.5

Allowed
as
child
 In
the
streets
 near
my
home
 Would
allow
child

77.5

76.8

72.9

73.5

74.9

69.3

74.7

63.6

50.5

45.2

46.1

44.0

51.4

38.6

45.5

42.5

Allowed
as
child

76.8

81.3

77.1

70.6

78.4

70.6

79.2

75.7

Would
allow
child

73.1

75.5

77.5

68.6

76.1

68.0

75.3

76.2

School
playing
 fields

Allowed
as
child

83.2

85.5

79.1

79.6

79.9

75.8

83.7

75.7

Would
allow
child

71.3

73.9

65.1

62.1

66.0

64.7

64.0

65.9

Outdoor
 adventure
 playground

Allowed
as
child

67.4

65.8

64.0

60.8

63.3

60.1

62.4

57.0

Would
allow
child

62.8

60.3

51.9

55.3

57.5

57.5

57.3

62.6

Allowed
as
child

61.5

62.6

60.5

69.3

68.0

69.9

74.2

75.7

Would
allow
child

36.5

37.4

33.7

37.2

42.9

43.8

44.4

57.9

Shrubland/
 fields/farm

Allowed
as
child

58.0

61.3

62.8

61.8

66.4

64.1

74.7

74.8

Would
allow
child

46.4

52.9

50.4

49.2

51.0

50.3

60.1

73.8

Riverside/
 creekside/
 pond

Allowed
as
child

55.4

61.0

54.7

57.6

62.9

60.1

64.0

63.6

Would
allow
child

36.1

34.2

31.4

34.3

29.7

32.7

41.0

39.3

Mountains/
 grassy
hills/
 wild
places

Allowed
as
child

62.8

60.6

58.1

62.8

66.4

62.7

67.4

65.4

Would
allow
child

45.5

43.2

40.3

43.7

44.0

45.1

48.9

59.3

Garden

Woods

Institute for Learning Innovation

44

Results
of
the
2010
C&NN
EC‐NES
National
Survey


Table
B.10:

Comparison
between
selected
state
 
and
total

Scale


































N=

Total
 Scale
 
 2138

CA
 Scale
 
 164

Healthiness

5.86

Cognitive/Emotional
Growth
 Emotional
Well‐being

CO
 Scale
 
 79

FL
 Scale
 
 109

MI
 Scale
 
 125

5.78

6.22

5.81

5.26

5.29

5.51

5.56

5.61

5.82

Enhanced
Skills

5.65

5.61

Appreciation
of
Nature

5.85

5.85

Priority*

6.30

Child
Safety*

4.90

Storytelling
 Effort/Risk*
 Intentionality

NH

NM

TX

88

Scale
 
 85

OH
 Scale
 
 148

5.81

5.85

5.82

5.77

5.86

5.29

5.17

5.30

5.05

5.18

5.28

5.50

5.42

5.61

5.50

5.45

5.60

5.86

5.58

5.67

5.70

5.57

5.62

5.69

6.08

5.86

5.88

5.85

5.67

5.83

5.87

6.28

6.57

6.10

6.31

6.23

6.34

6.35

6.36

4.80

5.25

4.66

5.11

5.00

5.01

4.86

4.91

5.07

4.78

5.24

4.98

5.09

5.23

4.85

5.11

5.16

5.85

5.79

6.08

5.79

5.88

5.82

5.94

5.83

5.90

6.11

6.08

6.31

6.01

6.19

6.00

6.05

6.05

6.16

Scale

Scale
 
 162

Notes:


 (1)
Values
based
on
responses
to
a
7
pointLikert‐type
scale
with
1
representing
strongly
disagree
and
7
representing
strongly
agree.

 (2)*
Designates
scale
contains
items
that
were
reverse
coded.

Table
B.11 (N=164)

Summated
Scale
Mean
Comparison,
Total
Mean
(N=2138)
to
State
of
California
Mean
 CA
 S.E.
 Mean

CA

CA
Scale
 
 164

.023

1.08

5.78

.088

1.12

.025

1.13

5.29

.089

1.13

5.56

.024

1.11

5.61

.085

1.10

5.65

.024

1.09

5.61

.086

1.10

Appreciation
of
Nature

5.85

.023

1.09

5.85

.082

1.05

Priority*

6.30

.025

1.15

6.28

.092

1.17

Child
Safety*

4.90

.024

1.10

4.80

.082

1.05

Storytelling

5.07

.031

1.42

4.78

.112

1.43

Effort/Risk

5.85

.024

1.09

5.79

.080

1.03

Intentionality

6.11

.023

1.08

6.08

.082

1.05

Total
 Scale

Total
S.E.
 Mean

Total
 Scale
SD

Scale
N=

2138

Healthiness

5.86

Cognitive/Emotional
Growth

5.26

Emotional
Well‐being
 Enhanced
Skills

SD

Notes:


 (1)
Values
based
on
responses
to
a
7
pointLikert‐type
scale
with
1
representing
strongly
disagree
and
7
representing
strongly
agree.

 (2)*
Designates
scale
contains
items
that
were
reverse
coded.

 Storytelling
 
significantly
different,
t(163)=‐2.60,
p.=.010

American
Attitudes
Toward
Children’s
Nature
Experiences

45
































































June
2010


Table
B.12:
 (N=79)

Summated
Scale
Mean
Comparison,
Total
Mean
(N=2138)
to
State
of
Colorado
Mean
 CO

CO

S.E.
 Mean

CO

79

1.08

6.22

.096

0.85

1.13

5.51

.136

1.21

.024

1.11

5.82

.115

1.03

.024

1.09

5.86

.103

0.91

5.85

.023

1.09

6.08

.125

1.11

6.30

.025

1.15

6.57

.097

0.87

Child
Safety*

4.90

.024

1.10

5.25

.109

0.97

Storytelling

5.07

.031

1.42

5.24

.178

1.58

Effort/Risk*

5.85

.024

1.09

6.08

.107

0.95

Intentionality

6.11

.023

1.08

6.31

.126

1.12

Total
 Scale

Total
S.E.
 Mean

Total
 Scale
SD

Scale



































































N=

2138

Healthiness

5.86

.023

Cognitive/Emotional
Growth

5.26

.025

Emotional
Well‐being

5.56

Enhanced
Skills

5.65

Appreciation
of
Nature
 Priority*

Scale

SD

Notes:


 (1)
Values
based
on
responses
to
a
7
pointLikert‐type
scale
with
1
representing
strongly
disagree
and
7
representing
strongly
agree.

 (2)*
Designates
scale
contains
items
that
were
reverse
coded.

 
 Priority


differences
statistically
significant,
t(78)=2.76,
p.=.007
 Healthiness


differences
statistically
significant,
t(78)=3.80,
p.=.000
 Effort/Risk


differences
statistically
significant
t(78)=2.19,
p=.032

 Enhanced
Skills


differences
statistically
significant
t(78)=2.00,
p.=.049
 Emotional
Well‐being


differences
statistically
significant
t(78)=2.25,
p.=.027

Table
B.13:

Summated
Scale
Mean
Comparison,
Total
Mean
(N=2138)
to
State
of
Florida
Mean
 (N=109)
 Total
 Scale

Total
S.E.
 Mean

Total
 Scale
SD

Scale


























































N=

2138

Healthiness

5.86

Cognitive/Emotional
Growth

5.26

Emotional
Well‐being
 Enhanced
Skills

FLS.E.
 Mean

FL
 SD

FL
Scale
 
 109

.023

1.08

5.81

.104

1.08

.025

1.13

5.29

.110

1.15

5.56

.024

1.11

5.50

.111

1.16

5.65

.024

1.09

5.58

.114

1.19

Appreciation
of
Nature

5.85

.023

1.09

5.86

.107

1.10

Priority*

6.30

.025

1.15

6.10

.125

1.31

Child
Safety*

4.90

.024

1.10

4.66

.105

1.10

Storytelling

5.07

.031

1.42

4.98

.143

1.49

Effort/Risk*

5.85

.024

1.09

5.79

.107

1.13

Intentionality

6.11

.023

1.08

6.01

.108

1.12

Notes:


 (1)
Values
based
on
responses
to
a
7
pointLikert‐type
scale
with
1
representing
strongly
disagree
and
7
representing
strongly
agree.

 (2)*
Designates
scale
contains
items
that
were
reverse
coded.

 Child
Safety
 
significantly
different
at
t(108)=‐2.26,
p=.026

Institute for Learning Innovation

46

Results
of
the
2010
C&NN
EC‐NES
National
Survey


Table
B.14:
 (N=125)

Summated
Scale
Mean
Comparison,
Total
Mean
(N=2138)
to
State
of
Michigan
Mean

Total

 Scale

Total

 S.E.
 Mean

Total
 Scale
SD

MI

 Scale

MI
 S.E.
 Mean

MI

Scale



























































N=

2138

125

Healthiness

5.86

.023

1.08

5.81

.090

1.00

Cognitive/Emotional
Growth

5.26

.025

1.13

5.17

.088

0.99

Emotional
Well‐being

5.56

.024

1.11

5.42

.096

1.08

Enhanced
Skills

5.65

.024

1.09

5.67

.075

.836

Appreciation
of
Nature

5.85

.023

1.09

5.88

.080

0.90

Priority*

6.30

.025

1.15

6.31

.100

1.11

Child
Safety*

4.90

.024

1.10

5.11

.104

1.16

Storytelling

5.07

.031

1.42

5.09

.121

1.36

Effort/Risk*

5.85

.024

1.09

5.88

.097

1.08

Intentionality

6.11

.023

1.08

6.19

.081

0.91

SD

Notes:


 (1)
Values
based
on
responses
to
a
7
pointLikert‐type
scale
with
1
representing
strongly
disagree
and
7
representing
strongly
agree.

 (2)*
Designates
scale
contains
items
that
were
reverse
coded.

 Difference
in
Child
Safety 
significantly
different
at
t(124)=2.02,
p=.046

Table
B.15:
 Summated
Scale
Mean
Comparison,
Total
Mean
(N=2138)
to
State
of
New
Hampshire
 Mean
(N=88)
 Total

 Scale

Total

 S.E.
 Mean

Total
 Scale
SD

Scale




























































N=

2138

NHScale 
 88

NH
 S.E.
 Mean

NH

Healthiness

5.86

.023

1.08

Cognitive/Emotional
Growth

5.26

.025

1.13

5.85

.124

1.16

5.30

.122

1.14

Emotional
Well‐being

5.56

.024

Enhanced
Skills

5.65

.024

1.11

5.61

.120

1.13

1.09

5.70

.113

1.06

Appreciation
of
Nature

5.85

Priority*

6.30

.023

1.09

5.85

.120

1.12

.025

1.15

6.23

.134

1.26

Child
Safety*

4.90

.024

1.10

5.00

.109

1.03

Storytelling

5.07

.031

1.42

5.23

.139

5.23

Effort/Risk*

5.85

.024

1.09

5.82

.120

1.13

Intentionality

6.11

.023

1.08

6.00

.122

1.14

SD

Notes:


 (1)
Values
based
on
responses
to
a
7
pointLikert‐type
scale
with
1
representing
strongly
disagree
and
7
representing
strongly
agree.

 (2)*
Designates
scale
contains
items
that
were
reverse
coded.

American
Attitudes
Toward
Children’s
Nature
Experiences

47
































































June
2010


Table
B.16:

Summated
Scale
Mean
Comparison,
Total
Mean
(N=2138)
to
State
of
New
Mexico
Mean
 (N=85)
 Total
 Scale

Total

 S.E.
 Mean

Total
 Scale
SD

NM
 Scale

NM
 S.E.
 Mean

NM

Scale




























































N=

2138

85

Healthiness

5.86

.023

1.08

5.82

.124

1.14

Cognitive/Emotional
Growth

5.26

.025

1.13

5.05

.151

1.39

Emotional
Well‐being

5.56

.024

1.11

5.50

.137

1.27

Enhanced
Skills

5.65

.024

1.09

5.57

.136

1.25

Appreciation
of
Nature

5.85

.023

1.09

5.67

.143

1.32

Priority*

6.30

.025

1.15

6.34

.132

1.22

Child
Safety*

4.90

.024

1.10

5.01

.117

1.08

Storytelling

5.07

.031

1.42

4.85

.162

1.49

Effort/Risk*

5.85

.024

1.09

5.94

.114

1.06

Intentionality

6.11

.023

1.08

6.05

.120

1.10

SD

Notes:


 (1)
Values
based
on
responses
to
a
7
pointLikert‐type
scale
with
1
representing
strongly
disagree
and
7
representing
strongly
agree.

 (2)*
Designates
scale
contains
items
that
were
reverse
coded.

Table
B.17:

Summated
Scale
Mean
Comparison,
Total
Mean
(N=2138)
to
State
of
Ohio
Mean

 (N=164)
 Total
 Scale

Total

 S.E.
 Mean

Total
 Scale
SD

OH

 Scale

OH
 S.E.
 Mean

OH

Scale




























































N=

2138

148

Healthiness

5.86

.023

1.08

5.77

.100

1.23

Cognitive/Emotional
Growth

5.26

.025

1.13

5.18

.090

1.09

Emotional
Well‐being

5.56

.024

1.11

5.45

.100

1.21

Enhanced
Skills

5.65

.024

1.09

5.62

.094

1.14

Appreciation
of
Nature

5.85

.023

1.09

5.83

.095

1.16

Priority*

6.30

.025

1.15

6.35

.093

1.13

Child
Safety*

4.90

.024

1.10

4.86

.077

0.94

Storytelling

5.07

.031

1.42

5.11

.120

1.46

Effort/Risk*

5.85

.024

1.09

5.83

.084

1.02

Intentionality

6.11

.023

1.08

6.05

.102

1.25

SD

Notes:


 (1)
Values
based
on
responses
to
a
7
pointLikert‐type
scale
with
1
representing
strongly
disagree
and
7
representing
strongly
agree.

 (2)*
Designates
scale
contains
items
that
were
reverse
coded.

Institute for Learning Innovation

48

Results
of
the
2010
C&NN
EC‐NES
National
Survey


Table
B.18:

 (N=162)

Summated
Scale
Mean
Comparison,
Total
Mean
(N=2138)
to
State
of
Texas
Mean
 TX

Scale

TX

TX

Total
 Scale

Total
S.E.
 Mean

Total
 Scale
SD

Scale




























































N=

2138

162

Healthiness

5.86

.023

1.08

5.86

.081

1.03

Cognitive/Emotional
Growth

5.26

.025

1.13

5.28

.083

1.05

Emotional
Well‐being

5.56

.024

1.11

5.60

.081

1.03

Enhanced
Skills

5.65

.024

1.09

5.69

.086

1.09

Appreciation
of
Nature

5.85

.023

1.09

5.87

.085

1.08

Priority*

6.30

.025

1.15

6.36

.079

1.01

Child
Safety*

4.90

.024

1.10

4.91

.085

1.08

Storytelling

5.07

.031

1.42

5.16

.109

1.39

Effort/Risk*

5.85

.024

1.09

5.90

.084

1.06

Intentionality

6.11

.023

1.08

6.16

.080

1.02

S.E.
 Mean

SD

Notes:


 (1)
Values
based
on
responses
to
a
7
pointLikert‐type
scale
with
1
representing
strongly
disagree
and
7
representing
strongly
agree.

 (2)*
Designates
scale
contains
items
that
were
reverse
coded.

American
Attitudes
Toward
Children’s
Nature
Experiences

49
































































June
2010


Turn static files into dynamic content formats.

Create a flipbook
Issuu converts static files into: digital portfolios, online yearbooks, online catalogs, digital photo albums and more. Sign up and create your flipbook.