PLACEBO EFFECT

Page 1

PLACEBO EFFECT CONDITIONING )

( EXPECTATION)

PLACEBO EFFECT

(


CONDIT


TIONING


01

PL ACEBO

EFFECT

“THE PHYSICIAN’S BELIEF IN THE TREATMENT AND THE PATIENT’S FAITH IN THE PHYSICIAN EXERT A MUTUALLY REINFORCING EFFECT; THE RESULT IS A POWERFUL REMEDY THAT IS ALMOST GUARANTEED TO PRODUCE AN IMPROVEMENT AND SOMETIMES A CURE.”

PLACEBO EFFECT

A placebo is a sham or simulated medical intervention that can produce a placebo effect. In medical research, placebos depend on the use of controlled and measured deception. Common placebos are inert tablets, sham surgery, and other procedures based on false information. In one common placebo procedure, a patient is given an inert pill, told that it may improve his / her condition, but not told that it is in fact iner t. Such an intervention may cause the patient to believe the treatment will change his /her condition; and this belief may produce a subjective perception of a therapeutic effect, causing the patient to feel their condition has improved. This phenomenon is known as the placebo effect. Placebos are widely used in medical research and medicine, and the placebo effect is a pervasive phenomenon; in fac t, it is par t of the response to any ac tive medical intervention. The placebo effect points to the importance of perception and the brain’s role in physical health. However, when used as treatment in clinical medicine (as opposed to laboratory research), the deception involved in the use of placebos creates tension between the Hippocratic Oath and the honesty of the doctor-patient relationship. The House of Commons of the United Kingdom Science and Technology Commit tee states that : “...prescribing placebos... usually relies on some degree of patient deception” and “prescribing pure placebos is bad medicine. Their effect is unreliable and unpredictable and cannot form the sole basis of any treatment on the NHS.” Since the publication of Henry K. Beecher’s The Powerful Placebo in 1955 the phenomenon has been considered to have clinically important effects. This view was notably challenged when in 2001 a systematic review of clinical trials concluded that there was no evidence of clinically important effects, except perhaps in the treatment of pain and continuous subjective outcomes. The article received a flurry of criticism, but the authors later published a Cochrane review with similar conclusions.Most studies have attributed the difference from baseline till the end of the trial to a placebo ef fect, but the reviewers examined studies which had both placebo and untreated groups in order to distinguish the placebo effect from the natural progression of the disease.


CONDITIONING

A placebo has been defined as “a substance or procedure ... that is objectively without specific activity for the condition being treated”. Under this definition, a wide variety of things can be placebos and exhibit a placebo effect. Pharmacological substances administered through any means can act as placebos, including pills, creams, inhalants, and injections. Medical devices such as ultrasound can act as placebos. Sham surgery, sham electrodes implanted in the brain, and sham acupuncture, either with sham needles or on fake acupuncture points, have all exhibited placebo effects. Bedding not treated to reduce allergies has been used as a placebo to control for treated bedding. The physician has even been called a placebo; a study found that patient recovery can be increased by words that suggest the patient “would be better in a few days”, and if the patient is given treatment, that “the treatment would certainly make him better” rather than negative words such as ““I am not sure that the treatment I am going to give you will have an effect”. The placebo effect may be a component of pharmacological therapies: Pain killing and anxiety reducing drugs that are infused secretly without an individual’s knowledge are less effective than when a patient knows they are receiving them. Likewise, the effects of stimulation from implanted electrodes in the brains of those with advanced Parkinson’s disease are greater when they are aware they are receiving this s timulation. Sometimes administering or prescribing a placebo merges into fake medicine.

E X P E C TAT I O N

02

The placebo effect has sometimes been defined as a physiological effect caused by the placebo, but Moerman and Jonas have pointed out that this seems illogical, as a placebo is an inert substance which does not directly cause anything. Instead they introduced the word “meaning response” for the meaning the brain associates with the placebo, which causes a physiological placebo effect. They propose that the placebo, which may be unethical, could be avoided entirely if doctors comfort and encourage their patients’ health. Ernst and Resch also attempted to distinguish between the “true” and “perceived” placebo effect, as they argued that some of the effects attributed to the placebo effect could be due to other factors. The placebo effect has been controversial throughout history. Notable medical organizations have endorsed it, but in 1903 Richard Cabot concluded that it should be avoided because it is deceptive. Newman points out the “placebo paradox”, - it may be unethical to use a placebo, but also unethical “not to use something that heals”. He suggests to solve this dilemma by appropriating the meaning response in medicine, that is make use of the placebo effect, as long as the “one administering ... is honest, open, and believes in its potential healing power.”


PL ACEBO

03

EFFECT

“NEARLY 60 PERCENT OF AMERICAN DOCTORS THINK THAT PRESCRIBING PLACEBO PILLS TO PATIENTS TO PROMOTE THEIR EXPECTATIONS IS ETHICALLY PERMISSIBLE.” 59%

59%

PERMISSBLE

APPROPRIATENESS OF RECOMENDING TREATMENT PRIMARILY TO PROMOTE PATIENTS’ EXPECTATIONS.

31%

3%

MEDICINE NOT TYPICALLY USED FOR YOUR CONDITION BUT MIGHT BENEFIT YOY.

7%

OBLIGATORY 3%

NE VER PERMISSIBLE 7%

PERMISSIBLE ONLY IN R ARE CIRCUMSTANCES 31%


CONDITIONING

E X P E C TAT I O N

04

68%

MEDICINE NOT TYPICALLY USED FOR YOUR CONDITION BUT MIGHT BENEFIT YOU. HOW PLACEBOS ARE TYPICALLY DESCRIBED TO PATIENTS.

18%

9%

MEDICINE WITH NO KNOWN EFFECT FOR YOUR CONDITION 9%

PL ACEBO 5%

5%

MEDICINE 18%


I SHALL FEEL BE AFTER I TAKE TH PILL.


L ETTER I HIS


07

PL ACEBO

EFFECT

SHAM SURGERY

Typical of the kind of flawed research methodology Hróbjartsson is referring to would be that of surgeon J. Bruce Moseley who performed fake knee surgery on eight of ten patients. (Fake surgery involves making an incision on the knee and stitching it up.) Six months after the surgery all the patients were satisfied customers. Rather than conclude that the patients didn’t need surgery or that the surgery was useless because in time the patients would have healed on their own, he and others concluded that the healing of the eight who did not have surgery was due to the placebo effect, while the two who had real surgery were better because of having had the operation. Irving Kirsch and Guy Sapirstein have been accused of making the same kind of methodological error in their controversial meta-analysis that found that antidepressants work by the placebo effect, rather than that antidepressants are unnecessary and useless. One more example should suffice to make the point that better designs of placebo studies are needed.

Forty years ago, a young Seattle cardiologist named Leonard Cobb conducted a unique trial of a procedure then commonly used for angina, in which doctors made small incisions in the chest and tied knots in two arteries to try to increase blood flow to the heart. It was a popular technique—90 percent of patients reported that it helped—but when Cobb compared it with placebo surgery in which he made incisions but did not tie off the arteries, the sham operations proved just as successful. The procedure, known as internal mammary ligation, was soon abandoned. Did Cobb show that this kind of surgery works by the placebo effect? Or did he show that the surgery was unnecessary because most of the patients would have healed on their own if nothing had been done? To rule out the natural history of a disease or regression to the mean, many researchers have used a third control group—those who receive no treatment at all. If the placebo group shows better results than the group getting nothing, then surely the placebo is effective. Hróbjartsson and Götzsche think most of these studies, too, are flawed, mainly due to having samples that are too small or due to patients who make reports aimed at pleasing the researcher.


CONDITIONING

Af ter the publication of the Hróbjar tsson and Götzsche study, Dr. John C. Bailar III said in an editorial that accompanied the study: “The shoe is on the other foot now. The people who claim there are placebo effects are going to have to show it.” The need, he said, is for large, rigorously designed studies that clearly define and measure effects of drugs and therapies versus placebos versus no intervention at all. These studies will have to clearly distinguish objective measurements (such as blood pressure, cholesterol levels, etc.) and subjective measurements (such as reports of pain or evaluative sensory observations by researchers, e.g., “I can see your tumor is smaller” or “I can see you are not as depressed as before”). The kind of study called for by Dr. Bailar has been done and several such studies are reviewed in chapter nine of R. Barker Bausell’s Snake Oil Science (2007): “How We Know That the Placebo Effect Exists.” One in particular is worth reviewing here. It was published in the Journal Pain t wo months af ter the Hróbjar tsson and Götzsche ar ticle. “Response expectancies in placebo analgesia and their clinical relevance” was the work of Antonella Pollo et all. and demonstrated that placebos can help people with serious pain. The following is from their abstract. Thoracotomized patients were treated with buprenorphine (a powerful pain reliever) on request for 3 consecutive days, together with a basal intravenous infusion of saline solution. However, the symbolic meaning of this basal infusion was changed in three different groups of patients. The first group was told nothing about any analgesic effect (natural history). The second group was told that the basal infusion was either a powerful painkiller or a placebo (classic double-blind administration). The third group was told that the basal infusion was a potent painkiller (deceptive administration). Therefore, whereas the analgesic treatment was exactly the same in the three groups, the verbal instructions about the basal infusion differed. The placebo effect of the saline basal infusion was measured by recording the doses of buprenorphine requested over the three-days treatment. We found that the doubleblind group showed a reduction of buprenorphine requests compared to the natural history group. However, this reduction was even larger in the deceptive administration group. Overall, after 3 days of placebo infusion, the first group received 11.55 mg of buprenorphine, the second group 9.15 mg, and the third group 7.65 mg. Despite these dose differences, analgesia was the same in the three groups. These results indicate that different verbal instructions about certain and uncertain expectations of analgesia produce different placebo analgesic effects, which in turn trigger a dramatic change of behaviour leading to a significant reduction of opioid intake. Several things are wor th noting about this experiment. The setting involves treatment being provided by medical personnel in a medical facility. This kind of setting usually involves a strong desire for recovery or relief on the part of the patient, as well as a belief that the treatment will be effective. The different verbal instructions about the basal IV would lead to different expectations. Belief, motivation, and expectation are essential to the placebo effect. Together, they are referred to as the subject-expectancy effect. Classical conditioning and suggestion by an authoritative healer seem to be triggering mechanisms for the placebo effect.

E X P E C TAT I O N

08


09

PL ACEBO

EFFECT

CONDITIONING AND EXPECTATION

Placebos exert an “expectancy” effect whereby an inert substance which is believed to be a drug has effects similar to the actual drug. Placebos can act similarly through classical conditioning, where a placebo and an actual stimulus are used simultaneously until the placebo is associated with the effect from the actual stimulus. Both conditioning and expectations play a role in placebo effect, and make different kinds of contribution. Conditioning has a longer lasting effect, and can effect earlier stages of information processing. The expectancy effect can be enhanced through factors such as the enthusiasm of the doctor, differences in size and color of placebo pills, or the use of other inventions such as injections. In one study, the response to a placebo increased from 44% to 62% when the doctor gave them with “warmth, attention, and confidence”. Expectancy effects have been found to occur with a range of substances. Those who think a treatment will work display a stronger placebo effect than those who do not, as evidenced by a study of acupuncture.

Bec ause the plac e bo e ffect is based u pon expectations and conditioning, the effect disappears if the patient is told that their expectations are unrealistic, or that the placebo intervention is ineffective. A conditioned pain reduction can be totally removed when its existence is explained. It has also been reported of subjects given placebos in a trial of anti-depressants, that “Once the trial was over and the patients who had been given placebos were told as much, they quickly deteriorated.” A placebo described as a muscle relaxant will cause muscle relaxation and if described as the opposite, muscle tension. A placebo presented as a stimulant will have this effect on heart rhy thm, and blood pressure, but when administered as a depressant, the opposite effect. The consumption of caffeine has been reported to cause similar effects even when decaffeinated coffee is consumed, although a 2003 study found only limited suppor t for this.Alcohol placebos can cause intoxication and sensimotor impairment. Perceived ergogenic aids can increase endurance, speed and weight-lifting ability, leading to the question of whether placebos should be allowed in sport competition. Placebos can help smokers quit. Perceived allergens which are not truly allergenic can cause allergies.Inventions such as psychotherapy can have placebo effects. The effect has been observed in the transplantation of human embryonic neurons into the brains of those with advanced Parkinson’s disease.


CONDITIONING

E X P E C TAT I O N

10

• ORANGE, RED AND OTHER HOT COLORED TABLETS WORK BETTER AS STIMULANTS. COOL COLORED ONES (BLUE, GREEN, PURPLE) WORK BETTER AS DEPRESSANTS. • BIG PILLS GENERALLY WORK BETTER THAN SMALL PILLS. • HIGHER PRICED PILLS WORK. BETTER THAN LOWER PRICED PILLS. • INJECTIONS WORK BETTER THAN TABLETS. • BRANDED TABLETS WORK BETTER THAN UNBRANDED TABLETS. Because placebos are dependent upon perception and expectation, various factors which change the perception can increase the magnitude of the placebo response. For example, studies have found that the color and size of the placebo pill makes a dif ference, with “hot- colored” pills working better as stimulants while “cool” colored pills work better as depressants. Capsules rather than tablets seem to be more effective, and size can make a difference. One researcher has found that big pills increase the effect while another has argued that the effect is dependent upon cultural background. More pills, branding, past experience, and high price increase the effect of placebo pills. Injection and acupuncture have larger effect than pills. Proper adherence to placebos have been found to decrease mortality. Motivation may contribute to the placebo effect. The ac tive goals of an individual changes their somatic experience by altering the detection and interpretation of expectation-congruent symptoms, and by changing the behavioral strategies a person pursues. Motivation may link to the meaning through which people experience illness and treatment. Such meaning is derived from the culture in which they live and which informs them about the nature of illness and how it responds to treatment. Research upon the placebo treatment of gastric and duodenal ulcers shows that this varies widely with society: those in Germany having a high rate placebo effect while those in Brazil a low one. Placebo effects in treating gastric ulcers is low in Brazil, higher in northern Europe (Denmark, Netherlands) and extremely high in Germany. But the placebo effect for hypertension is lower in Germany than elsewhere. Social observation can induce a placebo effect such when a person sees another having reduced pain following what they believe is a pain reducing procedure. The placebo effect can work selectively. If an analgesic placebo cream is applied on one hand, it will reduce pain only in that hand and not elsewhere on the body If a person is given a placebo under one name, and they respond, they will respond in the same way on a later occasion to that placebo under that name but not if under another.

GREEN PILLS reduces anxiety, adding more chill to the pill.

WHITE PILLS particularly those labeled “antacid”—are superior for soothing ulcers, even when they contain nothing

YELLOW PILLS Make the most effective antidepressants, like little doses of pharmaceutical

RED PILLS Can give you a more stimulating kick. Wake up, Neo.


11

BRAIN AND BODY

PL ACEBO

EFFECT

Functional imaging upon placebo analgesia shows that it links to the activation, and increased functional correlation between this activation, in the anterior cingulate, prefrontal, orbitofrontal and insular cor tices, nucleus accumbens, amygdala, the brainstem periaqueductal gray matter, and the spinal cord. These changes can ac t upon the brain’s early stages of information processing : research using evoked brain potentials upon painful laser pulses, for example, finds placebo effects upon the N2–P2, a biphasic negative–positive complex response, the N2 peak of which is at about 230 ms, and the P2 one at about 380 ms. They occur not only during placebo analgesia but after receiving the analgesic placebo (the areas are different here, and involve the medial prefrontal cortex, posterior parietal cortex and inferior parietal lobule). Different areas in the higher brain have different functions. The prefrontal involvement could be related to recalling the placebo and maintaining its cognitive presence in a “self-reinforcing feedback loop” (during pain an individual recalls having taken the placebo and reduced pain reinforces its status as an analgesic). The rostral anterior cingulate cortex (RACC) and its subcortical connectivity could be related to the expectation of potential pain stimuli. The higher brain works by regulating subcortical processes. High placebo responses link with enhanced dopamine and mu-opioid activity in the circuitry for reward responses and motivated behavior of the nucleus accumbens, and conversely, anti-analgesic nocebos responses were associated with deac tivation in this par t of the brain of dopamine and opioid release. (It has been known that placebo analgesia depends upon the release in the brain of endogenous opioids since 1978.) Such analgesic placebos activation changes processing lower down in the brain by enhancing the descending inhibition through the periaqueductal gray on spinal nociceptive reflexes, while the expectations of antianalgesic nocebos acts in the opposite way to block this. The brain is also involved in less studied ways upon nonanalgesic placebo effects:

• Parkinson’s disease: placebo relief is associated with the release of dopamine in the brain. • Depression: Placebos reducing depression affect many of the same areas that are activated by antidepressants with the addition of the prefrontal cortex. • Caffeine: placebo caffeinated coffee causes an increase in bilateral dopamine release in the thalamus. • Glucose: the expectation of an intravenous injection of glucose increases the release of dopamine in the basal ganglia of men (but not women). Present functional imaging upon placebo analgesia has been summarized as showing that the placebo response is “mediated by “top - down” proces ses dependent on frontal cortical areas that generate and maintain cognitive expectancies. Dopaminergic reward pathways may underlie these expectancies”. “Diseases lacking major ‘top-down’ or cortically based regulation may be less prone to placeborelated improvement”.


CONDITIONING

E X P E C TAT I O N

12

BLOOD SUPPLY TO THE SPINAL CORD: HORIZONTAL DISTRIBUTION

POSTERIOR

POSTERIOR SPINAL ARTERY

PERIPHER AL

CENTR AL ANTERIOR SPINAL ARTERY

ANTERIOR

CA SE ST U DY

This interesting study was conducted by researchers from the Germany’s University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf. After gaging participants’ pain threshhold by applying heat to their arms, researchers then treated them with what they told them was either a curing cream, or a “control cream.” Men who were treated with the fake cream, reported more pain after being initially treated than they did before the cream was applied. The researchers say that their study finding may lead one day to better treatments for people living with forms of chronic pain.


13

PL ACEBO

EFFECT


CONDITIONING

E X P E C TAT I O N

14

IT’S ALL IN YOUR MIND Some believe the placebo effect is purely psychological. Irving Kirsch, a psychologist at the University of Connecticut, believes that the effectiveness of Prozac and similar drugs may be attributed almost entirely to the placebo effect. He and Guy Sapirstein analyzed 19 clinical trials of antidepressants and concluded that the expectation of improvement, not adjustments in brain chemistry, accounted for 75 percent of the drugs’ effectiveness (Kirsch 1998). “The critical factor,” says Kirsch, “is our beliefs about what’s going to happen to us. You don’t have to rely on drugs to see profound transformation.” In an earlier study, Sapirstein analyzed 39 studies, done between 1974 and 1995, of depressed patients treated with drugs, psychotherapy, or a combination of both. He found that 50 percent of the drug effect is due to the placebo response. A person’s beliefs and hopes about a treatment, combined with their suggestibility, may have a significant biochemical effect, however. Sensory experience and thoughts can affect neurochemistry. The body’s neurochemical system affects and is affected by other biochemical systems, including the hormonal and immune systems. Thus, it is consistent with current knowledge that a person’s hopeful attitude and beliefs may be very important to their physical well-being and recovery from injury or illness. Perhaps this is why many people are dismayed when they are told that the effective drug they are taking is a placebo. This makes them think that their problem is “all in their mind” and that there is really nothing wrong with them. Yet, there are too many studies that have found objective improvements in health from placebos to support the notion that the placebo ef fect is entirely psychological. Doctors in one study successfully eliminated warts by painting them with a brightly colored, inert dye and promising patients the warts would be gone when the color wore of f. In a study of asthmatics, researchers found that they could produce dilation of the air ways by simply telling people they were inhaling a bronchodilator, even when they weren’t. Patients suffering pain after wisdom-tooth extraction got just as much relief from a fake application of ultrasound as from a real one, so long as both patient and therapist thought the machine was on. Fifty-two percent of the colitis patients treated with placebo in 11 different trials reported feeling better -- and 50 percent of the inflamed intestines actually looked better when assessed with a sigmoidoscope (“The Placebo Prescription” by Margaret Talbot, New York Times Magazine, January 9, 2000). It is unlikely that such effects are purely psychological. In fact, Martina Amanzio et al. (2001) demonstrated that “at least part of the physiological basis for the placebo ef fec t is opioid in nature” ( Bausell 2007: 160 ). We can be conditioned to release such chemical substances as endorphins, catecholamines, cortisol, and adrenaline. One reason, therefore, that people report pain relief from both acupuncture and sham acupuncture is that both are placebos that stimulate the opioid system.




17

PL ACEBO

EFFECT

Another popular belief is that a process of treatment that involves showing attention, care, affection, etc., to the patient subject, a process that is encouraging and hopeful, may itself trigger physical reactions in the body which promote healing. According to Dr. Walter A. Brown, a psychiatrist at Brown University:

THE PROCESS OF TREATMENT BELIEF

... there is certainly data that suggest that just being in the healing situation accomplishes something. Depressed patients who are merely put on a waiting list for treatment do not do as well as those given placebos. And—this is very telling, I think—when placebos are given for pain management, the course of pain relief follows what you would get with an active drug. The peak relief comes about an hour after it’s administered, as it does with the real drug, and so on. If placebo analgesia was the equivalent of giving nothing, you’d expect a more random pattern. Dr. Brown and others believe that the placebo effect is mainly or purely physical and due to physical changes that promote healing or feeling better. So, what is the explanatory mechanism for the placebo effect? Some think it is the process of administering it. It is thought that the touching, the caring, the attention, and other interpersonal communication that is part of the controlled study process (or the therapeutic setting), along with the hopefulness and encouragement provided by the experimenter/healer, affect the mood, expectations, and beliefs of the subject, which in turn triggers physical changes such as release of endorphins, catecholamines, cortisol, or adrenaline. The process reduces stress by providing hope or reducing uncertainty about what treatment to take or what the outcome will be. The reduction in stress prevents or slows down further harmful physical changes from occurring. The healing situation provokes a conditioned response. The patient’s been healed before by the doctor (or thinks she’s been healed before by the doctor) and expects to be healed again.


CONDITIONING

E X P E C TAT I O N

“A PROCESS THAT IS ENCOURAGING AND HOPEFUL, MAY ITSELF TRIGGER PHYSICAL REACTIONS IN THE BODY WHICH PROMOTE HEALING.”

18


19

SYMTOMS AND CONDITIONS

PL ACEBO

EFFECT

DEPRESSION

A meta-analysis in 1998 found that half of the effectiveness of anti-depressant medication is due to the placebo effect rather than the treatment itself. A meta-analysis in 2008 found that 79% of depressed patients receiving placebo remained well compared to 93% of those receiving antidepressants for the effect of placebos[clarification needed] (for 12 weeks after an initial 6–8 weeks of successful therapy). Another meta-analysis in 2002 found a 30% reduction in suicide and attempted suicide in the placebo groups compared to a 40% reduction in the treated groups. A 2002 article in The Washington Post titled “Against Depression, a Sugar Pill Is Hard to Beat” summarized research as follows: “In the majority of trials conducted by drug companies in recent decades, sugar pills have done as well as -- or better than -- antidepressants. Companies have had to conduct numerous trials to get two that show a positive result, which is the Food and Drug Administration’s minimum for approval. The makers of Prozac had to run five trials to obtain two that were positive, and the makers of Paxil and Zoloft had to run even more”.

“THE PLACEBO EFFECT OCCURS MORE STRONGLY IN SOME CONDITIONS THAN OTHERS. ONE STUDY FOUND PLACEBO EFFECTS ARE MOST LIKELY TO BE FOUND WITH THE PERIPHERAL ASPECTS OF DISEASE PROCESSES, RATHER THAN PROCESSES THAT REFLECT PHYSICAL DISEASE.”


CONDITIONING

E X P E C TAT I O N

20

PAIN

GASTRIC AND DUODENAL ULCERS

Placebo analgesia is more likely to work the more severe the pain. It can be effective: one study found for postoperative pain following the extraction of the third molar, that a saline injected while telling the patient it was a powerful painkiller was as potent as a 6–8 mg dose of morphine. Most research reports average reduction for a group of people, but this can be lower (some people do not respond). In one study using injection of capsaicin below the skin found that this reduced group average pain compared to no placebo by ~46% to ~57%. Another measure is the ability to endure pain. In one study, placebos increased this on average by about 3.5 minutes in the context of just under 14 minutes without it. The average strength of placebos upon pain on a visual analog scale is 2 out of 10 units. Individuals that respond to placebos show greater effects and can be 5 out of 10 units.

A meta-study of 31 placebo-controlled trials of the gastric acid secretion inhibitor drug cimetidine in the treatment of gastric or duodenal ulcers found that placebo treatments, in many cases, were as effective as active drugs: of the 1692 patients treated in the 31 trials, 76% of the 916 treated with the drug were “healed”, and 48% of the 776 treated with placebo were “healed”. These results were confirmed by the direct posttreatment endoscopy. It was also found that German placebos were “stronger” than others; and that, overall, different physicians evoked quite different placebo responses in the same clinical trial. Moreover, in many of these trials the gap between the active drugs and the placebo controls was “not because [the trials’ constituents] had high drug effectiveness, but because they had low placebo effectiveness”. In some trials, placebos were effective in 90% of the cases, whilst in others the placebos were only effective in 10% of the cases. It was argued that “what is demonstrated in these studies is not enhanced healing in drug groups, but reduced healing in placebo groups”. It was also noted the results of two studies (one conducted in Germany, the other in Denmark), which examined “ulcer relapse in healed patients” showed that the rate of relapse amongst those “healed” by the active drug treatment was five times that of those “healed” by the placebo treatment.

L I ST OF M E DICA L CON DI T IONS ADHD: adult, child Amalgam fillings: attributed symptoms (inert “chelation” therapy) Anxiety disorders Asthma (water aerosol inhalant) Asthma Autism: language and behavior problems Benign prostatic enlargement Binge eating disorder Bipolar mania Cough Crohn’s disease Depression (light treatment; low red light placebo) Depression Dyspepsia and gastric motility

Pain Panic disorders Parkinson’s disease Pathological gambling Premenstrual dysphoric disorder. Psoriatic arthritis Reflux esophagitis Restless leg syndrome Rheumatic diseases Sexual dysfunction: women Social phobia Third molar extraction swelling (sham ultra-sound) Ulcerative colitis Vulvar vestibulitis

Epilepsy Erectile dysfunction Food allergy: ability to eat ill-making foods Gastric and duodenal ulcers Headache Heart failure, congestive Herpes simplex Hypertension: mild and moderate Irritable bowel syndrome Migraine prophylaxis Multiple sclerosis Nausea: gastric activity Nausea: chemotherapy Nausea and vomiting: postoperative (sham acupuncture)


I SHALL FEEL GR AFTER I TAKE H SURGER


L REAT I HAVE RY.


PL ACEBO

23

EFFECT

MEAN STANDARDIZED IMPROVEMENT AS A FUNCTION OF INITIAL SEVERITY AND TREATMENT GROUP, INCLUDING ONLY TRIALS WHOSE SAMPLES HAD HIGH INITIAL SEVERITY

PALCEBO

CLINICALLY SIGNIFICANT DIFFERENCE

1.0 0.5 0.0

Improvement (d)

1.5

2.0

DRUG

24

28

32

Initial Severity (Baseline HRSD)

Drug improvement is portrayed as red triangles around their solid red regression line and placebo improvement as blue circles around their dashed blue regression line; the green shaded area indicates the point at which comparisons of drug versus placebo reach the NICE clinical significance criterion of d = 0.50. Plotted values are sized according to their weight in analyses.


CONDITIONING

E X P E C TAT I O N

24

CONTROLLED STUDIES

A Placebo-controlled study is a way of testing a medical therapy in which, in addition to a group of subje c ts that receives the treatment to be evaluated, a separate control group receives a sham “placebo” treatment which is specifically designed to have no real effect. Placebos are most commonly used in blinded trials, where subjects do not know whether they are receiving real or placebo treatment. Often, there is also a further “natural history” group that does not receive any treatment at all. The purpose of the placebo group is to account for the placebo ef fect, that is, ef fects from treatment that do not depend on the treatment itself. Such factors include knowing one is receiving a treatment, attention from health care professionals, and the expectations of a treatment’s effectiveness by those running the research study. Without a placebo group to compare against, it is not possible to know whether the treatment itself had any effect. Patients frequently show improvement even when given a sham or “fake” treatment. Such intentionally iner t placebo treatments can take many forms, such as a pill containing only sugar, a surger y where nothing is actually done, or a medical device (such as ultrasound) that is not actually turned on. Also, due to the body’s natural healing ability and statistical effects such as regression to the mean, many patients will get better even when given no treatment at all. Thus, the relevant question when assessing a treatment is not “does the treatment work?” but “does the treatment work better than a placebo treatment, or no treatment at all?” As one early clinical trial researcher wrote, “the first object of a therapeutic trial is to discover whether the patients who receive the treatment under investigation are cured more rapidly, more completely or more frequently, than they would have been without it.”[1]p.195 More broadly, the aim of a clinical trial is to determine what treatments, delivered in what circumstances, to which patients, in what conditions, are the most effective. Therefore, the use of placebos is a standard control component of most clinical trials which attempt to make some sort of quantitative assessment of the efficacy of medicinal drugs or treatments. Such a test or clinical trial is called a placebo-controlled study. Government regulatory agencies approve new drugs only after tests establish not only that patients respond to them, but also that their effect is greater than that of a placebo (by way of af fec ting more patients, by af fecting responders more strongly, or both). As a result, “placebocontrolled studies of ten are designed in such a way that disadvantages the placebo condition”.


25

THE GENECTIC CONNECTION

PL ACEBO

EFFECT


CONDITIONING

E X P E C TAT I O N

26

Because the placebo effect shows profound variability among individuals, some researchers have looked for evidence of a genetic predisposition to susceptibility to placebo effects. Andrew Leuchter et al. postulated that placebos act through central reward pathways modulated by monoamines, which are under strong genetic control. Their findings “support the hypothesis that genetic polymorphisms modulating monoaminergic tone are related to degree of placebo responsiveness in major depressive disorder.�* The researchers stressed that genetics is not the sole explanation for a placebo response, which is likely to be influenced by several biological and psychosocial factors.


27

PL ACEBO

THE ETHICAL DILEMMA The power of the placebo effect has led to an ethical dilemma. One should not deceive other people, but one should relieve the pain and suf fering of one’s patients. Should one use deception to benefit one’s patients? Is it unethical for a doctor to knowingly prescribe a placebo without informing the patient? If informing the patient reduces the effectiveness of the placebo, is some sort of deception warranted in order to benefit the patient? Some doctors think it is justified to use a placebo in those types of cases where a strong placebo effect has been shown and where distress is an aggravating factor.* Others think it is always wrong to deceive the patient and that informed consent requires that the patient be told that a treatment is a placebo treatment. Others, especially complementary and alternative medicine (CAM) practitioners, don’t even want to know whether a treatment is a placebo or not. Their attitude is that as long as the treatment is effective, who cares if it a placebo? While it may be unethical to knowingly package, prescribe, or sell placebos as magical cures, the CAM folks seem to think they are ethical because they really believe in their chi, meridians, yin, yang, prana, vata, pitta, kapha, auras, chakras, energies, spirits, succussion, natural herbs, water with precise and selective memory, subluxations, cranial and vertebral manipulations, douches and irrigations, body maps, divinities, and various unobservable processes that allegedly carry out all sorts of magical analgesic and curative functions.

EFFECT


CONDITIONING

E X P E C TAT I O N

ARE PLACEBO DANGEROUS? “SUCH THERAPIES MAY BE ENGENDERING NOTHING MORE THAN THE EXPECTATION THAT THEY WILL REDUCE PAIN BY ELABORATE EXPLANATIONS, PROMISES, AND CEREMONIES.”

While medical field may reject faith, prayer and “alternative” medical practices such as bioharmonics, chiropractic, and homeopathy, such practices may not be without their salutary effects. Clearly, they can’t cure cancer or repair a punctured lung, and they might not even prolong life by giving hope and relieving distress as is sometimes thought. But administering placebo therapies does involve interacting with the patient in a caring, attentive way, and this can provide some measure of comfor t. However, to those who say “what dif ference does it make why something works, as long as it seems to work” I reply that it is likely that there is something that works even better and might even be cheaper. Worse, some people might seek out a quack healer for a serious disorder that isn’t affected by the quack therapy but could be relieved or cured by scientific medicine. Furthermore, placebos may not always be beneficial or harmless.

Patients can become dependent on nonscientific practitioners who employ placebo therapies. Such patients may be led to believe they’re suffering from imagined “reactive” hypoglycemia, nonexistent allergies and yeast infections, dental filling amalgam “toxicity,” or that they’re under the power of extraterrestrials. And patients can be led to believe that diseases are only amenable to a specific type of treatment from a specific practitioner . The placebo can be an open door to quackery. R. Barker Bausell speculates that since complementary and alternative medicine ( CAM ) practitioners’ greatest asset is their nourishment of hope, “such therapies may be engendering nothing more than the expectation that they will reduce pain by elaborate explanations, promises, and ceremonies”. Packaging placebos is big business and is likely to get even bigger. The only thing that could slow down CAM atavism would be the sudden appearance of horrible side ef fects issuing from treatments like aura cleansings or homeopathic douches.

28


EXPECT


TATION



Turn static files into dynamic content formats.

Create a flipbook
Issuu converts static files into: digital portfolios, online yearbooks, online catalogs, digital photo albums and more. Sign up and create your flipbook.