Editorial Project

Page 1

Upper Six Jessica Awotwi Dadzie


Confused Octopus Just as in people, the drug stimulates the animals to behave more socially. How do you study the neuroscience of a creature whose brain bears little resemblance to that of humans? One approach, detailed yesterday (September 20) in Current Biology, is to see how the animal reacts to MDMA—also known as the street drug ecstasy. Johns Hopkins University researchers Gül Dölen and Eric Edsinger report that the drug induced social behavior in California two-spot octopuses (Octopus bimaculoides), much as it does in humans, suggesting similar roles for serotonin signaling in intelligent life throughout the animal kingdom. “I was absolutely shocked that it had this effect,” Judit Pungor, a neuroscientist at the University of Oregon who was not involved in the research, tells NPR. Octopuses are more closely related to snails than to humans, and their brains are organized very differently—they lack a cerebral cortex, for example. “It’s a little bit like studying alien intelligence,” Dölen tells National Geographic. “It can potentially tell us a lot about the ‘rules’ for building a nervous system that supports complex cognitive behaviors, without getting bogged down in the incidental organization of brains.”

The genome for the California twospot octopus was sequenced a few years ago, and in this study, Dölen and Edsinger found that it contains a gene similar to SLC6A4, which in humans encodes a serotonin transporter that binds to MDMA. That docking stimulates the release of serotonin, a neurotransmitter linked, among other things, to mood and social behavior. To explore how the octopus version of the receptor functions, the researchers borrowed seven of the animals from Woods Hole Marine Biological Laboratory and devised a behavioral test for them. After dunking them in either a saltwater bath or a solution of MDMA, they put the octopuses in an enclosure with a toy at one end and another, caged octopus at the other. Octopuses are not social animals, and before they received the drug, “[t]hey mashed themselves against one wall, very slowly extended one arm, touched the [other animal], and went back to the other side,” Dölen tells The Atlantic. But after the MDMA bath they spent more time near the other animal, and, she tells National Geographic, “they tended to hug the cage and put their

mouth parts on the cage. . . . This is very similar to how humans react to MDMA; they touch each other frequently.” To explore how the octopus version of the receptor functions, the researchers borrowed seven of the animals from Woods Hole Marine Biological Laboratory and devised a behavioral test for them. After dunking them in either a saltwater bath or a solution of MDMA, they put the octopuses in an enclosure with a toy at one end and another, caged octopus at the other. To explore how the octopus version of the receptor functions, the researchers borrowed seven of the animals from Woods Hole Marine Biological Laboratory and devised a behavioral test for them. After dunking them in either a saltwater bath or a solution of MDMA, they put the octopuses in an enclosure with a toy at one end and another, caged octopus at the other. “I was absolutely shocked that it had this effect,” Judit Pungor, a neuroscientist at the University of Oregon who was not involved in the research, tells NPR.


Confused Octopus Just as in people, the drug stimulates the animals to behave more socially.

How do you study the neuroscience of a creature whose brain bears little resemblance to that of humans? One approach, detailed yesterday (September 20) in Current Biology, is to see how the animal reacts to MDMA—also known as the street drug ecstasy. Johns Hopkins University researchers Gül Dölen and Eric Edsinger report that the drug induced social behavior in California two-spot octopuses (Octopus bimaculoides), much as it does in humans, suggesting similar roles for serotonin signaling in intelligent life throughout the animal kingdom. “I was absolutely shocked that it had this effect,” Judit Pungor, a neuroscientist at the University of Oregon who was not involved in the research, tells NPR. anything more than attraction.” “Octopuses really are the best example we have on Earth of a second intelligence,” San Francisco State University neuroscientist Robyn Crook tells the magazine. The similarities they show with humans in the new study indicate, she says, that “there are only so many ways to make an intelligent brain.”

Octopuses are more closely related to snails than to humans, and their brains are organized very differently—they lack a cerebral cortex, for example. “It’s a little bit like studying alien intelligence,” Dölen tells National Geographic. “It can

potentially tell us a lot about the ‘rules’ for building a nervous system that supports complex cognitive behaviors, without getting bogged down in the incidental organization of brains.” The genome for the California two-spot octopus was sequenced a few years ago, and in this study, Dölen and Edsinger found that it contains a gene similar to SLC6A4, which in humans encodes a serotonin transporter that binds to MDMA. That docking stimulates the release of serotonin, a neurotransmitter linked, among other things, to mood and social behavior. To explore how the octopus version of the receptor functions, the researchers borrowed seven of the animals from Woods Hole Marine Biological Laboratory and devised a behavioral test for them. After dunking them in either a saltwater bath or a solution of MDMA, they put the octopuses in an enclosure with a toy at one end and another, caged octopus at the other.

Octopuses are not social animals, and before they received the drug, “[t]hey mashed themselves against one wall, very slowly extended one arm, touched the [other animal], and went back to the other side,” Dölen tells The Atlantic. But after the MDMA bath they spent more time near the other animal, and, she tells National Geographic, “they tended to hug the cage and put their mouth parts on the cage. . . . This is very similar to how humans react to MDMA; they touch each other frequently.” One takeaway, Dölen tells NPR, is that “serotonin has been encoding social functions for a very, very long time. At least 500 million years ago, it started doing this function.” It’s possible the behavior could be explained by something other than social impulses, the University of Lethbridge’s Jennifer Mather, who was not involved in the study, tells The Atlantic. She suggests the drug could have affected the animals’ perception of chemical cues. “There’s no proof that it is


Confused Octopus Just as in people, the drug stimulates the animals to behave more socially.

How do you study the neuroscience of a creature whose brain bears little resemblance to that of humans? One approach, detailed yesterday (September 20) in Current Biology, is to see how the animal reacts to MDMA—also known as the street drug ecstasy. Johns Hopkins University researchers Gül Dölen and Eric Edsinger report that the drug induced social behavior in California two-spot octopuses (Octopus bimaculoides), much as it does in humans, suggesting similar roles for serotonin signaling in intelligent life throughout the animal kingdom. “I was absolutely shocked that it had this effect,” Judit Pungor, a neuroscientist at the University of Oregon who was not involved in the research, tells NPR. Octopuses are more closely related to snails than to humans, and their brains are organized very differently—they lack a cerebral cortex, for example. “It’s a little bit like studying alien intelligence,” Dölen tells National Geographic. “It can potentially tell us a lot about the ‘rules’ for building a nervous system that supports complex cognitive behaviors, without getting bogged down in the incidental organization of brains.” The genome for the California two-spot octopus was sequenced a few years ago, and in this study, Dölen and Edsinger found that it contains a gene similar to SLC6A4, which in humans encodes a serotonin transporter that binds to MDMA. That docking stimulates the release of serotonin, a neurotransmitter linked, among other things, to mood and social behavior. To explore how the octopus ver-

sion of the receptor functions, the researchers borrowed seven of the animals from Woods Hole Marine Biological Laboratory and devised a behavioral test for them. After dunking them in either a saltwater bath or a solution of MDMA, they put the octopuses in an enclosure with a toy at one end and another, caged octopus at the other. Octopuses are not social animals, and before they received the drug, “[t]hey mashed themselves against one wall, very slowly extended one arm, touched the [other animal], and went back to the other side,” Dölen tells The Atlantic. But after the MDMA bath they spent more time near the other animal, and, she tells National Geographic, “they tended to hug the cage and put their mouth parts on the cage. . . . This is very similar to how humans react to MDMA; they touch each other frequently.” One takeaway, Dölen tells NPR, is that “serotonin has been encoding social functions for a very, very long time. At least 500 million years ago, it started doing this function.” attraction.” “Octopuses really are the best example we have on Earth of a second intelligence,” San Francisco State University neuroscientist Robyn Crook tells the magazine. The similarities they show with humans in the new study indicate, she says, that “there are only so many ways to make an intelligent brain.”


Confused Octopus Just as in people, the drug stimulates the animals to behave more socially.

How do you study the neuroscience of a creature whose brain bears little resemblance to that of humans? One approach, detailed yesterday (September 20) in Current Biology, is to see how the animal reacts to MDMA—also known as the street drug ecstasy. Johns Hopkins University researchers Gül Dölen and Eric Edsinger report that the drug induced social behavior in California two-spot octopuses (Octopus bimaculoides), much as it does in humans, suggesting similar roles for serotonin signaling in intelligent life throughout the animal kingdom. “I was absolutely shocked that it had this effect,” Judit Pungor, a neuroscientist at the University of Oregon who was not involved in the research, tells NPR. Octopuses are more closely related to snails than to humans, and their brains are organized very differently—they lack a cerebral cortex, for example. “It’s a little bit like studying alien intelligence,” Dölen tells National Geographic. “It can potentially tell us a lot about the ‘rules’ for building a nervous system that supports complex cognitive behaviors, without getting bogged down in the incidental organization of brains.” The genome for the California two-spot octopus was sequenced a few years ago, and in this study, Dölen and Edsinger found that it contains

a gene similar to SLC6A4, which in humans encodes a serotonin transporter that binds to MDMA. That docking stimulates the release of serotonin, a neurotransmitter linked, among other things, to mood and social behavior. To explore how the octopus version of the receptor functions, the researchers borrowed seven of the animals from Woods Hole Marine Biological Laboratory and devised a behavioral test for them. After dunking them in either a saltwater bath or a solution of MDMA, they put the octopuses in an enclosure with a toy at one end and another, caged octopus at the other. Octopuses are not social animals, and before they received the drug, “[t]hey mashed themselves against one wall, very slowly extended one arm, touched the [other animal], and went back to the other side,” Dölen tells The Atlantic. But after the MDMA bath they spent more time near the other animal, and, she tells National Geographic, “they tended to hug the cage and put their mouth parts on the cage. . . . This is very similar to how humans react to MDMA; they touch each other frequently.” One takeaway, Dölen tells NPR, is that “serotonin has been encoding social functions for a very, very long time. At least 500 million years ago, it started doing this function.”

It’s possible the behavior could be explained by something other than social impulses, the University of Lethbridge’s Jennifer Mather, who was not involved in the study, tells The Atlantic. She suggests the drug could have affected the animals’ perception of chemical cues. “There’s no proof that it is anything more than attraction.” “Octopuses really are the best example we have on Earth of a second intelligence,” San Francisco State University neuroscientist Robyn Crook tells the magazine. The similarities they show with humans in the new study indicate, she says, that “there are only so many ways to make an intelligent brain.”


Confused Octopus Just as in people, the drug stimulates the animals to behave more socially.

How do you study the neuroscience of a creature whose brain bears little resemblance to that of humans? One approach, detailed yesterday (September 20) in Current Biology, is to see how the animal reacts to MDMA—also known as the street drug ecstasy. Johns Hopkins University researchers Gül Dölen and Eric Edsinger report that the drug induced social behavior in California two-spot octopuses (Octopus bimaculoides), much as it does in humans, suggesting similar roles for serotonin signaling in intelligent life throughout the animal kingdom. “I was absolutely shocked that it had this effect,” Judit Pungor, a neuroscientist at the University of Oregon who was not involved in the research, tells NPR. Octopuses are more closely related to snails than to humans, and their brains are organized very differently—they lack a cerebral cortex, for example. “It’s a little bit like studying alien intelligence,” Dölen tells National Geographic. “It can potentially tell us a lot about the ‘rules’ for building a nervous system that supports complex cognitive behaviors, without getting bogged down in the incidental organization of brains.”

The genome for the California two-spot octopus was sequenced a few years ago, and in this study, Dölen and Edsinger found that it contains a gene similar to SLC6A4, which in humans encodes a serotonin transporter that binds to MDMA. That docking stimulates the release of serotonin, a neurotransmitter linked, among other things, to mood and social behavior. To explore how the octopus version of the receptor functions, the researchers borrowed seven of the animals from Woods Hole Marine Biological Laboratory and devised a behavioral test for them. After dunking them in either a saltwater bath or a solution of MDMA, they put the octopuses in an enclosure with a toy at one end and another, caged octopus at the other. Octopuses are not social animals, and before they received the drug, “[t]hey mashed themselves against one wall, very slowly extended one arm, touched the [other animal], and went back to the other side,” Dölen tells The Atlantic. But after the MDMA bath they spent more time near the other animal, and, she tells National Geographic, “they tended to hug the cage

and put their mouth parts on the cage. . . . This is very similar to how humans react to MDMA; they touch each other frequently.” One takeaway, Dölen tells NPR, is that “serotonin has been encoding social functions for a very, very long time. At least 500 million years ago, it started doing this function.” It’s possible the behavior could be explained by something other than social impulses, the University of Lethbridge’s Jennifer Mather, who was not involved in the study, tells The Atlantic. She suggests the drug could have affected the animals’ perception of chemical cues. “There’s no proof that it is anything more than attraction.” “Octopuses really are the best example we have on Earth of a second intelligence,” San Francisco State University neuroscientist Robyn Crook tells the magazine. The similarities they show with humans in the new study indicate, she says, that “there are only so many ways to make an intelligent brain.”


Location We need a vacation. You run in my mind vacations I don’t mean anything much when I say I keep getting thirsty. We need a vacation.

INSTRUCTIONS USE TWO OF YOUR OWN PHOTOS FROM LOCATION AND CREATE YOUR OWN LAYOUT CHOOSE TEXT THAT YOU THINK WILL BE RELEVANT

S A

end me your location, let’s focus on communicating. ‘Cause I just need the time and place to come through (place to come through). Send me your location. Let’s ride the vibrations, I don’t need nothing else but you. t times I wonder why I fool with you but this is new to me, this is new to you initially, I didn’t wanna fall for you. Gather my attention it was all for you, so don’t. Take advantage, don’t leave my heart damaged to understand that things go a little bit better when you plan it...oh. Send me your location, let’s focus on communicating. ‘Cause I just need the time and place to come through (place to come through). Send me your location. Let’s ride the vibrations, I don’t need nothing else but you. I don’t wanna fall in love off of subtweets, so let’s get personal, I got a lot of cool spots that we can go tell me what’s the move and I got you. ’m only acting like this ‘cause I like you. Just give me the vibe to slide then. Oh, I might make you mine by the night, and send me your location, let’s focus on communicating. ‘Cause I just need the time and place to come through (place to come through). Send me your location let’s ride the vibrations. I don’t need nothing else but you. Ride, ride, ride, come and vibe with me tonight. I don’t need nothing else but you, (I don’t need nothing else but you). Ride, ride, ride, come and vibe with me tonight. I don’t need nothing else but you. Nothing else but you.

I


Turn static files into dynamic content formats.

Create a flipbook
Issuu converts static files into: digital portfolios, online yearbooks, online catalogs, digital photo albums and more. Sign up and create your flipbook.