Understanding of Patterns, Streaks and Independence by Grade School Children KEYWORDS: Teaching; Representativeness heuristic; Law of small numbers; Coin tossing.
Carolyn Ridgway Merced, California, USA. e-mail: ridgways@mindspring.com
Derry Ridgway SRA International, Research Triangle Park, North Carolina, USA. Summary Using sequences of coin flips as a model of serial independent events, we asked schoolchildren in grades 1 through 5 to estimate the likelihood of the next flip. Most children in each grade expected short patterns to continue.
䉬 INTRODUCTION 䉬
A
dults and older students may invoke errorprone heuristics to arrive at incorrect estimates for the likelihood of chance events (Kahneman et al. 1982). In this article, we use a simple coinflipping experiment to examine an error-prone heuristic related to the independence of serial events in schoolchildren from grade 1 through grade 5 (US ages 6–11). We characterize the error-prone reasoning as follows: The subject reviews a short sequence of independent random events with the goal of predicting the likelihood of the next event. Subjects well grounded in the notion of independence will not attempt to extrapolate any pattern evident in the short sequence to the next outcome. Subjects less well grounded in independence may assign likelihoods by erroneously reasoning from the results of the immediately preceding outcomes. The expectation that brief samples will closely reflect the theoretical overall set of outcomes for longer sequences was called the “representativeness heuristic” by Kahneman and Tversky (1979) and has also been referred to as the “law of small numbers” (Burns 2004; Burns and Corpus 2004).
The concept of independence is integral to understanding probabilities associated with more than a single event (Barnes 1998; Meletiou-Mavrotheris and Lee 2002; Metz 1998a; Polaki 2002; Rubel 2006; Shaughnessy 1992; Tarr and Jones 1997). However, independence does not appear in the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (NCTM) curriculum standards until secondary school (NCTM 2000), and the teaching of independence in school children is given little attention 34
• Teaching Statistics.
Volume 32, Number 2, Summer 2010
(Barnes 1998; Hawkins and Kapadia 1984; Meletiou-Mavrotheris and Lee 2002; Metz 1998a; Shaughnessy 1992; Tarr and Jones 1997). Using a sequence of coin flips as our model for random events, we asked approximately 100 schoolchildren to estimate the likelihood (heads more likely, tails more likely, heads and tails equally likely) for the seventh flip in a series after reviewing the results of the first six flips. We hypothesized that in our experimental model, young students would exhibit the representativeness heuristic and, in the absence of curricular attention, older students would continue to exhibit this error even as other nondeterministic reasoning skills increased.
䉬 METHODS 䉬 The subjects were ninety-eight students from grades 1 to 5 in a 400-student public elementary school in North Carolina. At the time of the study, mathematics teaching followed the 2003 K-12 Mathematics Standard Course of Study for the Public Schools of North Carolina from the State Board of Education (current standards at http://www.ncpublicschools. org/curriculum/mathematics), based on the NCTM Principles and Standards for the Teaching of Mathematics (2000) (http://standards.nctm.org). Both standards include five strands: Number and Operation, Measurement, Geometry, Data Analysis and Probability, and Algebra. For this study, the © 2010 The Authors Journal compilation © 2010 Teaching Statistics Trust